Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity AI transcript and summary - episode of podcast Huberman Lab
Go to PodExtra AI's episode page (Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity) to play and view complete AI-processed content: summary, mindmap, topics, takeaways, transcript, keywords and highlights.
Go to PodExtra AI's podcast page (Huberman Lab) to view the AI-processed content of all episodes of this podcast.
Huberman Lab episodes list: view full AI transcripts and summaries of this podcast on the blog
Episode: Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity
Author: Scicomm Media
Duration: 02:18:56
Episode Shownotes
In this episode, my guest is Dr. Zachary Knight, Ph.D., a professor of physiology at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF), and Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI) investigator. We discuss how the brain controls our sense of hunger, satiety, and thirst. He explains how dopamine levels impact our cravings
and eating behavior (amount, food choices, etc) and how we develop and can change our food preferences and adjust how much we need to eat to feel satisfied. We discuss factors that have led to the recent rise in obesity, such as interactions between our genes and the environment and the role of processed foods and food combinations. We also discuss the new class of medications developed for the treatment of obesity and diabetes, including the GLP-1 agonists semaglutide (Ozempic, Wegovy) and tirzepatide (Mounjaro). We discuss how these medications work to promote weight loss, the source of their side effects, and the newer compounds soon to overcome some of those side effects, such as muscle loss. Dr. Knight provides an exceptionally clear explanation for our sense of hunger, thirst, and food cravings that translates to practical knowledge to help listeners better understand their relationship to food, food choices, and meal size to improve their diet and overall health. For show notes, including referenced articles and additional resources, please visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman
BetterHelp: https://betterhelp.com/huberman
Eight Sleep: https://eightsleep.com/huberman
Waking Up: https://wakingup.com/huberman
LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman
Timestamps 00:00:00 Dr. Zachary Knight 00:02:38 Sponsors: BetterHelp, Helix Sleep & Waking Up 00:07:07 Hunger & Timescales 00:11:28 Body Fat, Leptin, Hunger 00:17:51 Leptin Resistance & Obesity 00:20:52 Hunger, Food Foraging & Feeding Behaviors, AgRP Neurons 00:30:26 Sponsor: AG1 00:32:15 Body Weight & Obesity, Genes & POMC Neurons 00:39:54 Obesity, Genetics & Environmental Factors 00:46:05 Whole Foods, Ultra-Processed Foods & Palatability 00:49:32 Increasing Whole Food Consumption, Sensory Specific Satiety & Learning 00:58:55 Calories vs. Macronutrients, Protein & Salt 01:02:23 Sponsor: LMNT 01:03:58 Challenges of Weight Loss: Hunger & Energy Expenditure 01:09:50 GLP-1 Drug Development, Semaglutide, Ozempic, Wegovy 01:19:03 GLP-1 Drugs: Muscle Loss, Appetite Reduction, Nausea 01:23:24 Pharmacologic & Physiologic Effects; GLP-1 Drugs, Additional Positive Effects 01:30:14 GLP-1-Plus Development, Tirzepatide, Mounjaro, AMG 133 01:34:49 Alpha-MSH & Pharmacology 01:40:41 Dopamine, Eating & Context 01:46:01 Dopamine & Learning, Water Content & Food 01:53:23 Salt, Water & Thirst 02:03:27 Hunger vs. Thirst 02:05:46 Dieting, Nutrition & Mindset 02:09:39 Tools: Improving Diet & Limiting Food Intake 02:14:15 Anti-Obesity Drug Development 02:17:03 Zero-Cost Support, Spotify & Apple Follow & Reviews, YouTube Feedback, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter Disclaimer
Summary
In this episode of the 'Huberman Lab' podcast, Dr. Andrew Huberman speaks with Dr. Zachary Knight about the brain's mechanisms governing hunger, satiety, and thirst. They explore the role of dopamine in cravings and dissect how genetic predisposition and environmental factors, like processed foods, contribute to obesity. Dr. Knight explains innovative pharmacological treatments, such as GLP-1 agonists for obesity and diabetes management. They discuss the role of neural circuits and hormones like leptin in hunger regulation and examine how altering food perceptions can affect eating behavior. The episode offers practical insights into maintaining a healthy relationship with food and outlines advancements in obesity and diabetes treatment.
Go to PodExtra AI's episode page (Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity) to play and view complete AI-processed content: summary, mindmap, topics, takeaways, transcript, keywords and highlights.
Full Transcript
00:00:00 Speaker_00
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. My guest today is Dr. Zachary Knight.
00:00:18 Speaker_00
Dr. Zachary Knight is a professor of physiology at the University of California, San Francisco, and an investigator with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
00:00:26 Speaker_00
For those of you that don't know, Howard Hughes Medical Investigators are selected from an extremely competitive pool of applicants and have to renew in order to maintain their investigatorship with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute every five years or so, placing him in the most elite of categories with respect to research scientists.
00:00:48 Speaker_00
His laboratory focuses on homeostasis in particular, what drives our sense of hunger, what drives our sense of thirst, and what controls thermoregulation, which is the ability to maintain body temperature within a specific, safe range.
00:01:02 Speaker_00
Today we mainly focus on hunger. Dr. Zachary Knight explains the biological mechanisms for craving food, for consuming food,
00:01:10 Speaker_00
And believe it or not, you have brain circuits that actually determine how much you're likely to eat even before you take your very first bite.
00:01:17 Speaker_00
And he explains the biological mechanisms for satiety, that is, the sense that one has had enough of a particular food or food group. Dr. Knight also explains the role of dopamine in food craving and consumption.
00:01:29 Speaker_00
which I think everybody will find very surprising because it runs counter-current to most people's understanding of what dopamine does in the context of eating and other cravings.
00:01:38 Speaker_00
Today's discussion also includes a deep dive into GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide, and the novel class of drugs such as Ozempic and Monjaro and other related compounds that are now widespread in use for the reduction in body weight.
00:01:53 Speaker_00
Dr. Knight explains how GLP-1 was first discovered and how these drugs were developed, how they work, and importantly, why they work, and how that is leading to the next generation of so-called diet drugs or drugs to treat obesity, diabetes, and related syndromes.
00:02:09 Speaker_00
We also discussed thirst and the intimate relationship between water consumption and food consumption. And we also talk about the relationship between sodium intake, water intake, and food intake.
00:02:19 Speaker_00
By the end of today's conversation, you will have learned a tremendous amount about the modern understanding of hunger, thirst, and salt intake, as well as this modern class of drugs, such as ozempic and related compounds, all from a truly world-class investigator in the subjects of researching hunger, thirst, and thermal regulation.
00:02:38 Speaker_00
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
00:02:43 Speaker_00
It is however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
00:02:55 Speaker_00
Our first sponsor is BetterHelp. BetterHelp offers professional therapy with a licensed therapist carried out entirely online. I've been doing weekly therapy for well over 30 years. Initially, I didn't have a choice.
00:03:07 Speaker_00
It was a condition of being allowed to stay in high school, but quickly I realized that therapy is an extremely important component to our overall health.
00:03:15 Speaker_00
In fact, I consider doing regular therapy as important as getting regular cardiovascular exercise and resistance training, which of course I also do every week. Now, there are essentially three things that great therapy provides.
00:03:27 Speaker_00
First of all, it requires that you have a really good rapport with the therapist, somebody that you can trust and talk to about what's really going on in your life.
00:03:35 Speaker_00
And of course, an excellent therapist will provide you support in moving towards the things that are going to grow your life in the best ways.
00:03:42 Speaker_00
And third, and this is the one that people often overlook, an expert therapist is somebody who can really provide you useful insights that would not otherwise be obvious to you.
00:03:51 Speaker_00
With BetterHelp, they make it very easy to find the therapist with whom you can have those three essential and highly effective components. If you'd like to try BetterHelp, you can go to betterhelp.com slash Huberman to get 10% off your first month.
00:04:04 Speaker_00
Again, that's betterhelp.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by Eight Sleep. Eight Sleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling, heating, and sleep tracking capacity.
00:04:15 Speaker_00
Now, I've spoken many times before on this podcast about the critical need for us to get adequate amounts of quality sleep each night. One of the best ways to ensure a great night's sleep is to control the temperature of your sleeping environment.
00:04:26 Speaker_00
And that's because in order to fall and stay deeply asleep, your body temperature actually has to drop by about one to three degrees.
00:04:32 Speaker_00
And in order to wake up feeling refreshed and energized, your body temperature actually has to increase by about one to three degrees.
00:04:39 Speaker_00
Eight Sleep makes it incredibly easy to control the temperature of your sleeping environment by allowing you to program the temperature of your mattress cover at the beginning, middle, and end of the night.
00:04:49 Speaker_00
I've been sleeping on an Eight Sleep mattress cover for well over three years now, and it has completely transformed my sleep for the better. Eight Sleep recently launched their newest generation pod cover, the Pod 4 Ultra.
00:05:00 Speaker_00
The Pod 4 Ultra has improved cooling and heating capacity, higher fidelity sleep tracking technology, and it also has snoring detection that remarkably will automatically lift your head a few degrees to improve your airflow and stop your snoring.
00:05:13 Speaker_00
If you'd like to try an Eight Sleep mattress cover, you can go to eightsleep.com slash Huberman to save $350 off their Pod 4 Ultra. Eight Sleep currently ships to the USA, Canada, UK, select countries in the EU and Australia.
00:05:26 Speaker_00
Again, that's eightsleep.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by Waking Up. Waking Up is a meditation app that offers hundreds of guided meditation programs, mindfulness trainings, yoga nidra sessions, and more.
00:05:40 Speaker_00
I started practicing meditation when I was about 15 years old, and it made a profound impact on my life.
00:05:46 Speaker_00
And by now, there are thousands of quality peer-reviewed studies that emphasize how useful mindfulness meditation can be for improving our focus, managing stress and anxiety, improving our mood, and much more.
00:05:58 Speaker_00
In recent years, I started using the Waking Up app for my meditations because I find it to be a terrific resource for allowing me to really be consistent with my meditation practice.
00:06:07 Speaker_00
Many people start a meditation practice and experience some benefits, but many people also have challenges keeping up with that practice.
00:06:13 Speaker_00
What I and so many other people love about the Waking Up app is that it has a lot of different meditations to choose from, and those meditations are of different durations.
00:06:21 Speaker_00
So it makes it very easy to keep up with your meditation practice, both from the perspective of novelty, You never get tired of those meditations.
00:06:28 Speaker_00
There's always something new to explore and to learn about yourself and about the effectiveness of meditation. And you can always fit meditation into your schedule, even if you only have two or three minutes per day in which to meditate.
00:06:40 Speaker_00
I also really like doing yoga nidra or what is sometimes called non-sleep deep rest for about 10 or 20 minutes, because it is a great way to restore mental and physical vigor without the tiredness that some people experience when they wake up from a conventional nap.
00:06:53 Speaker_00
If you'd like to try the waking up app, please go to wakingup.com slash Huberman, where you can access a free 30 day trial. Again, that's wakingup.com slash Huberman to access a free 30 day trial. And now for my discussion with Dr. Zachary Knight.
00:07:07 Speaker_00
Dr. Zachary Knight, welcome. Great to be here. Today, we're going to talk about hunger, appetite, thirst, other motivated behaviors, the role of dopamine,
00:07:19 Speaker_00
the vagus nerve, these are terms and topics that a lot of people hear nowadays and for which there's a ton of interest, but just to march us in sequentially, could you describe some of what's happening in the brain and or body as we get hungry, decide what to eat and then decide that we've had enough to eat
00:07:43 Speaker_00
I think most people just assume that, okay, that my stomach's full is what we say. I've had enough, or we self-regulate it for some other reason, caloric restriction or monitoring in some cases.
00:07:57 Speaker_00
What's happening in the brain in terms of the circuitries? And what have you discovered about what that process looks like in terms of its kind of universality across people, and then maybe how it sometimes differs between people?
00:08:11 Speaker_01
Okay, there's a lot in that that I'll try to unpack.
00:08:14 Speaker_00
And I can remind you of some of the nuances. In other words, as a biologist, as a neuroscientist, how do you think about this thing that we call hunger and feeding? Absolutely, absolutely.
00:08:21 Speaker_01
So I think at a very high level, a good way to think about the regulation of food intake by the brain is that
00:08:27 Speaker_01
There's two systems, a short-term system and a long-term system that are primarily localized to different parts of the brain, operate on different timescales, one on the timescale of a meal, so 10, 20 minutes, and the other on the timescale of sort of weeks to months to years and tracks levels of body fat.
00:08:46 Speaker_01
And these two systems sort of interact so that these short-term behaviors we do eating are matched to our long-term need for energy. And so, I think one of the initial experiments that really led to this idea is this great experiment by Harvey Grill.
00:09:04 Speaker_01
about 50 years ago. It's called the decerebrate rat. And so essentially what he did was he made a cut in the rat brain.
00:09:11 Speaker_01
So he took these rats in the lab, made a cut so that he separated the brainstem, so the most posterior part of the brain, from the entire forebrain. Basically got rid of 80% of the rat's brain. So he's basically creating these zombie rats.
00:09:24 Speaker_01
All they have is a brainstem. And asked, what can these rats still do? And as you might imagine, they can't do a lot of things, right? Because they basically have lost most of their brain.
00:09:33 Speaker_01
But he discovered that one thing they can still do is regulate the size of a meal. And so- Very informative experiment.
00:09:42 Speaker_01
And so, and you have to be careful how we talk about this, because the way this meal works is you have to actually put food into their mouth and then they'll swallow it as you put food into their mouth.
00:09:52 Speaker_01
But eventually at some point they'll start spitting it out. And that basically is an indication that in some sense they're becoming sated and they're,
00:10:00 Speaker_01
just using the brainstem that they have left, they're able to sense those signals from the gut and drive the termination of a meal.
00:10:07 Speaker_01
And he did other experiments showing that many of these signals that come from the gut, gastric stretch, hormones that come from your intestine in response to food intake, like CCK, these desirabate rats just have a brainstem.
00:10:18 Speaker_01
If you inject those or manipulate the gut in those ways, it can, in an appropriate way, change how much the rat eats. Now what can't the rat do when it doesn't have a forebrain?
00:10:29 Speaker_01
And the thing it can't do is it can't respond to longer term changes in energy need. Meaning, if you fast the rat for a couple days, this decerebrate rat, then start putting food in its mouth, the amount that it eats doesn't change.
00:10:42 Speaker_01
So basically it doesn't eat a larger meal the way you would if you were fasted for several days and then refed.
00:10:48 Speaker_01
And that experiment, along with other evidence, has led to the idea that in the brainstem, and then the most posterior part of your brain, there are neural circuits
00:10:56 Speaker_01
that control sort of a meal and then the timescale of 10 minutes or 20 minutes deciding when a meal should end.
00:11:02 Speaker_01
And in the forebrain, primarily in the hypothalamus, there are neural circuits that then track what is my overall level of energy reserves? What is my level of body fat? Things that would fluctuate on timescale of say days when you're fasting.
00:11:13 Speaker_01
And those forebrain centers feedback to talk to the brainstem and modulate those brainstem circuits that are controlling the size of a meal to sort of match these two timescales.
00:11:22 Speaker_01
So that's at the highest level how I think about the neural circuitry that controls feeding. There's obviously a lot more going on underneath that.
00:11:29 Speaker_00
Fascinating. You mentioned body fat and that somehow the brain is tracking the amount of body fat. That caught my ear because while it makes total sense, I'd like to know how that happens if we happen to know the mechanism.
00:11:46 Speaker_00
And the second question is why body fat and not body fat and muscular mass or body fat and overall body weight? What is being signaled between body fat and the brain that allows the brain to track body fat?
00:12:01 Speaker_00
And why do you think body fat is the critical signal? I realize it represents an energy reserve, but certainly there are other things about the bodily state that are important.
00:12:10 Speaker_01
Yeah, well, there are certainly other things about the bodily state that are important and there are other things about physiology definitely that are regulated other than body fat. But body fat is unique because it represents this energy reserve.
00:12:21 Speaker_01
So the neural circuitry that regulates eating behavior is in some ways very unique because it has this reserve of energy. So we also study thirst in my lab and drinking and you don't have a reserve of water in your body, right?
00:12:33 Speaker_01
And that's true for basically everything else. For fat, we have this reserve of energy.
00:12:39 Speaker_01
And so it's very important that the brain know how much remains and then adjust behavior in coordinates with that so that you know how urgent it is to get the next meal.
00:12:53 Speaker_01
And so the thought is that the major signal of the level of body fat that we have is leptin. It's this hormone. It was discovered, it was cloned in 1994 actually by my postdoctoral advisor, a scientist named Jeff Friedman at Rockefeller University.
00:13:05 Speaker_01
although its history goes back way before 1994.
00:13:07 Speaker_01
So the story behind Leptin is that there's a facility called Jackson Labs that you, I'm sure, are familiar with in Maine, that since the 1920s has been raising mice and selling them to academics, basically, who study physiology and behavior.
00:13:25 Speaker_01
And so they breed thousands of mice. It's sort of a nonprofit organization that distributes mice to the scientific community.
00:13:31 Speaker_01
And at some point in the 1950s, they spontaneously, just because they were breeding so many mice, they came across some spontaneous mutations, mutant mice that were extremely fat, like the fattest mice they had ever seen.
00:13:42 Speaker_01
These mice just eat constantly. They're just enormous, three times the size of a normal mouse. And it's all body fats. They're just these huge fat mice. And they came across several different mutant strains.
00:13:57 Speaker_01
that all had the same phenotype, in the sense that they were all extremely fat, all extremely hyperphagic, but they could tell, even in the 1950s, that these mutations were on different chromosomes.
00:14:07 Speaker_01
They didn't know anything about how to identify the genes at that point, that was just science fiction, but they knew that there were chromosomes and they were on different chromosomes.
00:14:14 Speaker_01
And so they labeled one obese, one of these mouse strains obese, and the other one diabetes, but they were basically the same. And so people wondered for a long time, well, what's going on in these mice?
00:14:24 Speaker_01
Then there was a scientist at Jackson Labs, Doug Coleman, who had the idea, what if we do an experiment where we connect the circulations of these two different strains of obese mice and test the hypothesis that maybe there's a circulating factor, a hormone, that is produced by one of these strains and that controls appetite?
00:14:42 Speaker_01
Because at that point insulin was known, glucagon was known, there were some hormones that were known that were involved in metabolism, so it was logical that there could be a hormone that perhaps regulates body fat levels.
00:14:52 Speaker_01
And what they found, which was remarkable, when you attach the OB strain to the DB strain, so you basically connect their circulation, so hormones are transmitted between the two, the OB mouse, that strain dramatically loses weight.
00:15:07 Speaker_01
In fact, within a couple of weeks, it looks like a normal mouse. It just stops eating, it loses almost all of its body fat, and it essentially, in all aspects, becomes a normal mouse. The DB mouse, nothing really happens.
00:15:17 Speaker_01
It still remains obese, it still remains hyperphagic. And based on just that piece of data, Doug Coleman hypothesized that what was going on is these two mutations were mutations in a hormone and a receptor.
00:15:29 Speaker_01
The OB mouse had a mutation in the hormone that comes from fat, so it couldn't produce this hormone that comes from fat and signals to the brain how much fat you have. And the DB mouse has a mutation in the receptor, so it can't sense the hormone.
00:15:44 Speaker_01
And it was just an idea, it was a hypothesis. But in the 1980s, as technology advanced, as molecular biology had been invented, it became possible to clone genes.
00:15:56 Speaker_01
A number of people tried to identify what are the genetic mutations that are occurring in these mice that make them so obese. And Jeff basically cloned leptin and showed that in fact, Doug was exactly right.
00:16:06 Speaker_01
The OB mutation is a mutation in this hormone, leptin. And later, Millennium Pharmaceuticals showed that the DB mutation is in fact a receptor. And it was an important discovery for a couple of reasons.
00:16:20 Speaker_01
One, because this OB gene is just expressed in fat. It's exclusively expressed in adipose tissue. And how much it's expressed is directly proportional to how much body fat you have.
00:16:32 Speaker_01
So as you gain weight, the expression of this hormone increases in a linear manner, and then it's secreted into the blood. So the level of leptin in your blood is a direct readout of your body fat reserves.
00:16:44 Speaker_01
This receptor for leptin, leptin receptor, the functional form of it is expressed almost exclusively in the brain. And it's expressed in all of the brain regions that we knew from previous work were important for appetite.
00:16:56 Speaker_01
So basically the expression of this receptor gives you a map in the brain of the neurons that control hunger. And so what happens is basically when you lose weight, the levels of leptin in your blood fall because basically you've lost adipose tissue.
00:17:09 Speaker_01
The absence of that hormone sends a signal to all these neurons that have leptin receptors in the brain. They're not getting that signal that I'm starving. And basically that initiates this entire homeostatic response to starvation.
00:17:20 Speaker_01
So a big part of that is obviously increased hunger, but it's also decreased energy expenditure, decreased body temperature, even decreased fertility because you don't wanna reproduce if you're starving. Less spontaneous movement.
00:17:34 Speaker_01
Less spontaneous movement, all of this.
00:17:38 Speaker_01
And so the thought is, which I think is absolutely correct, is that this hormone leptin is part of this negative feedback loop from the fat to the brain that basically tells you about your level of body fat reserves and how urgent it is to find the next meal.
00:17:50 Speaker_00
Fascinating. As I recall, Amgen Pharmaceuticals owned the patent for leptin in hopes that it would become the blockbuster diet drug.
00:17:59 Speaker_00
The logic being that if you were to take this hormone somehow or activate this pathway, that the brain would be tricked into thinking that there was more body fat, more energy reserves than there was, and then people would,
00:18:12 Speaker_00
basically be less hungry, eat less and lose body fat. What happened with that? Do we know why it did not work?
00:18:19 Speaker_01
Yeah. So that's a great question. So there was a lot of excitement when leptin was cloned because it was thought basically we've cured obesity.
00:18:26 Speaker_01
There was an auction for the patent, Amgen won, I think it was something like $20 million upfront payment plus royalties, which at the time was, I mean, it still is a lot of money, but even more money.
00:18:35 Speaker_00
Nowadays, it would be a drop in the ocean compared to what companies will invest into potential diet drugs.
00:18:41 Speaker_01
Exactly, but at the time, and still a lot of money today. And they did a clinical trial, gave obese people leptin, subcutaneous injections of this hormone, and they didn't lose a lot of weight. And the question was why?
00:18:57 Speaker_01
And so what was subsequently revealed is that the challenge with leptin is that individuals who are obese do not have low levels of leptin for the most part. They actually have high levels of leptin.
00:19:08 Speaker_01
And so what they have is a state of leptin resistance. So it's analogous to someone who has type two diabetes. It's not because they lack insulin, it's because they actually have over time a high level of insulin.
00:19:17 Speaker_01
And so target tissue stop responding to insulin. And the thought is that it's the same way in obesity and leptin. Now, subsequently,
00:19:24 Speaker_01
they went back and did a reanalysis of that clinical trial and asked, what if you take all of these people and stratify them according to their starting leptin level?
00:19:33 Speaker_01
So some people have relatively low levels of leptin, some have higher, some have really high levels of leptin. And then ask, if we reanalyze the data, how effective is leptin?
00:19:43 Speaker_01
And as you might expect, the people with the lowest levels of leptin, they lost the most weight when you gave them this drug. And the people with the highest levels of leptin lost the least weight.
00:19:51 Speaker_01
So there is a rationale there for why, for a scenario in which leptin could work, either among the subset of people who just have, for some reason, lower levels of leptin.
00:20:01 Speaker_01
These aren't people with mutations like the OB mouse, they have some leptin, they just don't have unusually high levels. Or alternatively, after weight loss. So after you've lost a lot of weight, your leptin levels plummet, they become very low.
00:20:13 Speaker_01
And that part of the reason, it's a big part of the reason it's so difficult to keep weight off is because those leptin levels are so low.
00:20:19 Speaker_01
And so it's been thought for a long time that that is a scenario where treating people with leptin could be really useful to help them keep the weight off. Why it never made it as a drug for that application, I really don't understand.
00:20:32 Speaker_01
It has something to do, I think, with the pharmaceutical industry, with the economics, with a bunch of other issues that aren't necessarily scientific.
00:20:39 Speaker_01
But I think there's still in the future is a possibility that it could come back for that indication, especially now that we have these GLP-1 drugs, and now there's just millions of people losing so much weight.
00:20:48 Speaker_01
and perhaps they want to transition to a different kind of drug to keep the weight off.
00:20:52 Speaker_00
We are definitely going to talk about GLP-1, Ozempic, and some of the related compounds in a few minutes.
00:20:58 Speaker_00
But before we do that, I'd love to get to this issue of what's happening in the brain as we get hungry, approach a meal, decide what to eat, and decide when we've had enough.
00:21:11 Speaker_00
are there separate circuitries or at least separate neurons for each of those steps?
00:21:15 Speaker_00
And if you would, could you walk us through what that process looks like since we do it every day, most people do it every day unless they're fasting, multiple times per day.
00:21:24 Speaker_00
What's going on in our brain and body as we think about and approach a meal, consume a meal and decide enough?
00:21:32 Speaker_01
Sure, so there are different neurons that are preferentially involved in different aspects of those processes. So I think people often divide feeding behavior, and many other kinds of motivated behaviors into appetitive and consummatory phases.
00:21:47 Speaker_01
So appetitive is the phase of the behavior where you're, for example, searching for food. It's foraging. It's all the actions that lead up to the actual behavior itself, which then we call the consummatory phase.
00:22:00 Speaker_01
That's actually putting the food in your mouth and eating it.
00:22:02 Speaker_01
And the general thought is that these forebrain circuits in the hypothalamus are more important, particularly in the hypothalamus, but other parts of the forebrain as well, are more important for the appetitive phase.
00:22:14 Speaker_01
And the brainstem circuits are more important for the consummatory phase, the actual putting it in your mouth and licking, chewing, swallowing, and all of that. Within the hypothalamus, there's a population of neurons called AGRP neurons.
00:22:24 Speaker_01
So it's just an acronym, AGRP, and it stands for agouti-related peptide, but it doesn't really matter. They're absolutely critical for that appetitive phase, for the searching for food, for the desire to find food and consume it when you're hungry.
00:22:39 Speaker_00
Sorry, just to touch on the AGRP neurons and this appetitive phase, are they known to connect to areas of the brain and body that stimulate the desire to move?
00:22:48 Speaker_00
Because I think about when I get hungry, if I'm at my desk or something, I need to get up and find food. I need to walk to lunch or go to the refrigerator. Are they somehow linked to the circuits promote locomotion?
00:23:01 Speaker_01
Well, they have to promote those things, but they're not directly linked to any of those circuits. They're linked directly to other forebrain circuits involved in motivation.
00:23:10 Speaker_01
So the way we think, we think about, you know, what these kinds of neurons like HRP neurons are doing, they're not directly talking to the motor circuits to tell you to move your legs or arms to pick up the sandwich or whatever.
00:23:21 Speaker_01
They're rather creating this general problem that the animal has to solve, which is that I'm hungry, I need to get food. It would be really great if I could have a sandwich.
00:23:30 Speaker_01
And then the animal uses all of its mental capacities, right, to solve that problem. So they're just there to set the goal, not so much to direct the solution.
00:23:38 Speaker_01
And so, but these AGRP neurons, yeah, there are a few thousand neurons at the base of the hypothalamus. So basically the most ventral, the most bottom part of the forebrain.
00:23:48 Speaker_01
So tiny population of cells, but outsized importance for the control of feeding behavior. So if you stimulate these cells in a mouse or a rat that's not hungry, the animal will voraciously eat like it's starving.
00:24:00 Speaker_01
If you silence these cells, animals will starve to death. So you can basically give them food. They just won't eat it voluntarily until basically you have to euthanize them because they've lost so much weight.
00:24:11 Speaker_01
And the activity of these AGRP neurons is thought to track the body's need for energy.
00:24:18 Speaker_01
One reason that's thought is that they express these receptors for leptin, this hormone that I was just talking about that comes from fat and signals the level of body fat reserves. And leptin inhibits AGRP neurons.
00:24:31 Speaker_01
So as you might expect, if you have lots of body fat, then a neuron that expresses, that controls hunger should be less active than if you have very little body fat. So that's one mechanism by which leptin controls hunger.
00:24:45 Speaker_01
We in my lab have investigated the role of these AGRP neurons from a slightly different perspective, which is, and this relates to your question about what happens when we approach food, when we start a meal.
00:24:58 Speaker_01
And to ask, what are their activity patterns? What is the natural sort of firing of this population of neurons when an animal eats a meal?
00:25:05 Speaker_01
It's a very basic question, something I think we've wanted to know for a long time, was not really addressable until about 10 years ago because the technology didn't exist, because these are such a tiny population of cells so deep in the brain.
00:25:18 Speaker_01
So one of the very first experiments we did in my lab was to investigate that, to ask for the first time, what happens to these AGRP neurons when an animal eats? And so one of my first graduate students, Yuming Chen,
00:25:28 Speaker_01
He used a technology called fiber photometry, which allows us to put a fiber optic into the mouse's brain. So then we could record fluorescence from these AGRP neurons, which we could use as a readout of their activity.
00:25:39 Speaker_01
It's basically using a calcium sensor. So calcium is a surrogate for neural activity. And one of the very first experiments he did, he said, let's make the animal hungry. These AGRP neurons will be very active because the animal's hungry.
00:25:51 Speaker_01
And then let's give it some food and see what happens during a meal.
00:25:54 Speaker_01
And our expectation was that these AGRP neurons would gradually decline in activity as the animal eats, and levels of hormones in the blood start changing, feeding back to inhibit these neurons. What we found was really surprising.
00:26:07 Speaker_01
I remember when he made this discovery, basically him running into my office and saying, Zach, I gave the mouse a piece of food, but the weirdest thing happened. The neurons shut off almost immediately. And I said, Ming, you've made a mistake. It's OK.
00:26:17 Speaker_01
You're just starting off in graduate school. This happens. Go back and repeat the experiment, and then we'll discuss it. But he did several times. He said he knows that every single time I do this happens.
00:26:25 Speaker_01
I give a hungry mouse food, and the AGRP neurons, within just a few seconds, their activity has greatly diminished, back to the level it would be in a fed mouse, even before they take the first bite of food.
00:26:36 Speaker_01
And so Yiming then went to do a series of experiments to try to understand what was going on.
00:26:40 Speaker_01
And what he basically showed by changing the kind of food he gave them or the accessibility of the food or how hungry the mouse was and measuring the response of these AGRP neurons was that what the neurons were doing was predicting.
00:26:53 Speaker_01
The mouse looks at the food, it looks at how palatable it is, imagines how hungry the mouse is, how accessible it is. And then within a few seconds, these neurons predict how much food the mouse is going to eat in the forthcoming meal.
00:27:04 Speaker_01
And so essentially these neurons know how much the mouse is going to eat before the mouse even takes the first bite.
00:27:09 Speaker_01
And you can show this by a very simple analysis in which you give the mouse different foods and you look at how much these AGRP neurons drop when the mouse sees and smells the food.
00:27:18 Speaker_01
And then you plot that against, this drop happens in three seconds, four seconds, something like that. Then you look at how much does the mouse go on to eat in the next 30 minutes. You can just draw a straight line.
00:27:28 Speaker_01
So this was one of the first results from my lab, and it was really surprising to all of us, and I think everyone.
00:27:32 Speaker_01
But it illustrated a theme that we've now seen again and again, which is that these circuits that control internal state, control things like hunger and thirst, what they're constantly doing is predicting the future.
00:27:42 Speaker_01
They can sense these signals from the body that tell you about what's happened, but those signals are slow.
00:27:47 Speaker_01
And you don't want to wait 20 minutes from the food that you ingested to reach your stomach and then slowly start entering your intestine to figure out what was the nutrient content of the meal.
00:27:56 Speaker_01
You want to try to figure that out as soon as you can, right?
00:28:00 Speaker_01
And so the animals learn presumably through just experience that, okay, something that smells like this and looks like this, it has about this many calories and I know I'm this hungry, so I'm gonna eat about this much.
00:28:08 Speaker_01
And then that information is all transmitted to these circuits to start the process of satiation before the meal begins.
00:28:15 Speaker_00
Is it satiation or it's ceasing of foraging so that the animal, or if I translate to a person, decides, okay, now I'm going to consume this sandwich, this package of food?
00:28:26 Speaker_01
Yeah, that's a great question. So we don't fully know the answer.
00:28:30 Speaker_01
So one interpretation of the data I just showed you is exactly what you said, is that what these neurons do is they control foraging alone, they don't control eating, and so this is perfect.
00:28:38 Speaker_01
You see the food, you know that it's got enough calories, the neurons shut off, and then you stay there and eat it. You transition from this appetitive to this consummatory phase.
00:28:46 Speaker_01
But that doesn't seem to be the whole explanation, because if you artificially stimulate these neurons, so prevent that drop from ever happening, just stimulate them continually, the mouse will just sit there and eat.
00:28:56 Speaker_01
So you can't fully separate, although we'd like to make this distinction between repetitive and consummatory, and we know that in different parts of the brain, there's more important for one versus the other, the reality is that the entire behavior is linked, and you can't fully separate them.
00:29:09 Speaker_01
So there's a number of ideas about what this means. So one idea that I just mentioned is that starting the process of satiety before the meal begins.
00:29:17 Speaker_01
Another idea, which you mentioned, which could be part of the answer, is that it is reducing this repetitive drive and allowing the transition to consummatory behavior.
00:29:27 Speaker_01
Another idea is that, and I call these ideas because we don't really fully know the answer yet for exactly what the purpose is, and in biology it's always hard to answer why something happens.
00:29:35 Speaker_01
You can figure out what happens, but then you can, the reason why it evolved that way is challenging. Another idea is it's involved in these what we call cephalic phase responses that are necessary to prepare you for a meal, right?
00:29:47 Speaker_01
So the famous example of this is Pavlov, right, basically trains the dog to associate the ringing of the bell with the presentation of food and then eventually the ringing of the bell alone causes the dog to salivate in the absence of any food.
00:30:01 Speaker_01
And salivation is one example of a cephalic phase response. The purpose of that is to have enzymes in your mouth that basically are gonna digest the food and get them there right before you need them.
00:30:10 Speaker_01
But there's all sorts of other things, like basically the secretion of insulin occurs in response to food cues, changes in gastric acid, gut motility, all these things are getting ready for the meal to happen.
00:30:20 Speaker_01
And so another idea is it could be part of that. But probably it's doing all of these things.
00:30:26 Speaker_00
As many of you know, I've been taking AG1 for more than 10 years now, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring this podcast. To be clear, I don't take AG1 because they're a sponsor. Rather, they are a sponsor because I take AG1.
00:30:38 Speaker_00
In fact, I take AG1 once and often twice every single day, and I've done that since starting way back in 2012. There is so much conflicting information out there nowadays about what proper nutrition is.
00:30:50 Speaker_00
But here's what there seems to be a general consensus on.
00:30:53 Speaker_00
Whether you're an omnivore, a carnivore, a vegetarian, or a vegan, I think it's generally agreed that you should get most of your food from unprocessed or minimally processed sources, which allows you to eat enough, but not overeat, get plenty of vitamins and minerals, probiotics, and micronutrients that we all need for physical and mental health.
00:31:11 Speaker_00
Now, I personally am an omnivore and I strive to get most of my food from unprocessed or minimally processed sources.
00:31:17 Speaker_00
But the reason I still take AG1 once and often twice every day is that it ensures I get all of those vitamins, minerals, probiotics, et cetera, but it also has adaptogens to help me cope with stress.
00:31:28 Speaker_00
It's basically a nutritional insurance policy meant to augment, not replace quality food. So by drinking a serving of AG1 in the morning, and again in the afternoon or evening, I cover all of my foundational nutritional needs.
00:31:40 Speaker_00
And I, like so many other people that take AG1, report feeling much better in a number of important ways, such as energy levels, digestion, sleep, and more.
00:31:49 Speaker_00
So while many supplements out there are really directed towards obtaining one specific outcome, AG1 is foundational nutrition designed to support all aspects of wellbeing related to mental health and physical health.
00:32:00 Speaker_00
If you'd like to try AG1, you can go to drinkag1.com slash Huberman to claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs with your order plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's drinkag1.com slash Huberman. It's so interesting.
00:32:16 Speaker_00
I have a number of questions, but I think the one that I'll put at the top of the list is the other night we were out to dinner in New York and I was very hungry. I hadn't eaten much that day and I was looking forward to a nice steak.
00:32:30 Speaker_00
They brought out bread, French bread, it was French restaurant. I took one bite, I realized it was absolutely delicious French bread. The butter was fantastic. And so I had some bread and butter, which I love.
00:32:41 Speaker_00
Then they brought more and then they started bringing out, I don't know who ordered them because I didn't, appetizers. And I realized that this was going to be a much more extensive calorically dense meal. And suddenly my appetite for the appetizers,
00:32:56 Speaker_00
it sort of went down because I knew there was more food coming, right? Had I not known that there was more food coming, I think I would have consumed more of the appetizers, which also looked great.
00:33:06 Speaker_00
So clearly there's something going on with these AGRP neurons at the moment. You're sort of integrating based on new information. On the other end of the spectrum, I did a solo episode about eating disorders and anorexia nervosa in particular.
00:33:20 Speaker_00
And one of the things that I learned from experts in that field, the psychiatrist who worked on this and the scientists who work on this, is that people with anorexia are unbelievably tuned to the caloric content of food.
00:33:38 Speaker_00
That their visual system and presumably other systems have become like almost hyper-accurate calculators of the amount of calories in food. They've devoted a lot of cognition to it. It sometimes can border on or be placed within the obsessive realm.
00:33:55 Speaker_00
but that they see food and they can tell you a tremendous amount about the caloric amounts with these foods, even food combinations with a very small margin of error. And that drives in that condition, obviously a food avoidance.
00:34:14 Speaker_00
So I have to assume that these AGRP neurons are involved in this kind of thing. One represents a regulation in the case of the example I gave and in the other case,
00:34:22 Speaker_00
but let's just call it what it is because anorexia nervosa is the most deadly of the psychiatric conditions, sadly. A pathologic dysregulation, a maladaptive dysregulation. So what is known about these AGRP neurons in humans?
00:34:35 Speaker_00
Meaning, do they exist in humans? Presumably they express the leptin receptor. It sounds like they are able to integrate information, both cognitive based on immediate experience, visual olfactory, but also a lot of prior experience.
00:34:54 Speaker_00
You know, a hamburger patty, I can't tell you how many calories it has. All I know is that it's mostly protein and some fat. You know, what are these neurons doing? What do they have access to?
00:35:06 Speaker_00
They sound like, you know, when anytime I hear about hypothalamus, I think very basic drives, but you're talking about a pretty sophisticated analysis of a real time,
00:35:14 Speaker_00
event that is driving fairly nuanced behavioral decisions and updating that, which is a big deal. We're both neuroscientists, but for everyone listening and watching, this is a big deal.
00:35:27 Speaker_00
This is as nuanced as deciding whether or not somebody is friend or foe or deciding whether or not you you like a movie or you don't. I mean, this is some pretty sophisticated processing. This isn't eat, don't eat, or eat less, eat more.
00:35:41 Speaker_00
These aren't switches. These are dials. Exactly.
00:35:45 Speaker_01
Yeah, so there's a lot there. I'll try to unpack that. So the first thing I would say is they are present in humans and humans do, humans have HRP neurons. Human HRP neurons express the leptin receptor. And we think the functions are very similar.
00:36:00 Speaker_01
So one of the nice things actually about studying these kinds of things, like basic mechanisms of hunger, thirst, because these things are so important for survival, they've been under really strong selection, right?
00:36:10 Speaker_01
And so many of the components of these systems are genetically hardwired, meaning these are cell types that have a single purpose, in this case to control hunger. They're labeled by specific genes and those are conserved through evolution.
00:36:22 Speaker_01
We also know that this pathway, this AGRP neuron pathway, is important in humans due to human genetics. So just to add a little bit more information here, there's a companion set of neurons called POMC neurons that promotes satiety.
00:36:37 Speaker_01
So they're sort of the yin and yang of hunger. AGRP neurons promote hunger, POMC neurons promote satiety. They're intermingled in the same part of the hypothalamus. They're axons that project to the exact same downstream brain regions.
00:36:52 Speaker_01
then it's thought that these two neurons compete with each other to control appetite.
00:36:57 Speaker_01
And that competition occurs through neuropeptides that they release, one of which is an agonist for a downstream receptor, and the other one of which is an antagonist.
00:37:06 Speaker_01
We know from human genetics that among severely obese people, mutations in this pathway, AGRP, POMC neurons, and their direct downstream targets, are quite common.
00:37:18 Speaker_00
So- So is it fair to say that some amount of obesity is genetic in nature at the level of neuronal firing or circuitry?
00:37:29 Speaker_01
I think a lot of body weight regulation is genetic. It's highly heritable. There's a question of how much of it is due to single genes. And the number of people quote, and this is among people who are severely obese.
00:37:40 Speaker_01
So not just people who you've seen someone who's overweight, but people have sort of syndromes where they're very obese from a very young age. Among those people, something on the order of 10% have mutations in this pathway.
00:37:52 Speaker_01
And it can either be this hormone, POMC, or an enzyme within those cells that processes POMC into the right form, or in the down, and this is the most common mutation, in the downstream receptor for POMC, it's called the melanocortin-4 receptor.
00:38:10 Speaker_01
And so among the severely obese, people who have sort of genetically inherited severe obesity from childhood, something under 10% have mutations in this pathway. So it's very clear that this pathway is involved in body weight regulation in humans.
00:38:24 Speaker_01
Most obesity, although there is a very strong genetic component, is not associated with single gene mutations like this. It's associated with effects of many mutations.
00:38:35 Speaker_01
But we know that even in that sort of polygenic obesity that has many different genetic causes, that the brain is important.
00:38:44 Speaker_01
And one of the reasons we know that is if you look at the genes through genetic association studies that have been associated with body weight, and there's been lots of genetic association studies trying to find mutations that are associated with whether you're lean or obese.
00:38:55 Speaker_01
something on the order of a thousand genes have been linked to body weight regulation. And the vast majority of those are expressed in the brain.
00:39:02 Speaker_01
They're highly enriched for brain processes, which makes sense because body weight is controlled by food intake, right? And the brain controls behavior and also the brain controls energy expenditure.
00:39:10 Speaker_01
So maybe it's not so surprising, but it's clear that mutations in genes in the brain are important for body weight, and which is consistent with the results of twin studies. So if you look at monozygotic versus dizygotic twins,
00:39:24 Speaker_01
the estimates for the heritability of body weight is something on the order of 80%.
00:39:28 Speaker_00
We should explain monozygotic, dizygotic, which I've talked about before on the podcast, just to brush people off.
00:39:33 Speaker_01
Just identical versus fraternal twins, basically. And so, and by comparing their, basically their body weight when they become adults, you can get a sense for how much of this is genetic versus environmental.
00:39:46 Speaker_01
And something on the order of 80% is thought to, the variation between individuals is thought to have a genetic component.
00:39:54 Speaker_00
Wow, I don't think most people appreciate that. And a lot of the debate we hear nowadays is because there are things that people can do to lose body fat, exercise, eat differently, et cetera, maybe embrace pharmacology if that's appropriate.
00:40:12 Speaker_00
There seems to be this, to me, silly debate as to whether or not people should be eating better and exercising or Assuming that all of the obesity they might have arises through genetic causes and therefore take a prescription drug.
00:40:28 Speaker_00
I mean, why wouldn't it be a combination of things? Like to me, it just seems like, why wouldn't people embrace all of this? some or all of the tools that they could afford and that are safe for them.
00:40:38 Speaker_00
So I just wanna get that out there because the moment this comes up, people start thinking, oh, well, the moment we assign a genetic source to something, we're removing personal responsibility.
00:40:47 Speaker_00
But of course, there are people, I know people who have struggled with their weight their entire lives.
00:40:52 Speaker_00
for whom some of these new pharmaceuticals like Ozempic have provided them the opportunity to finally be able to lose weight and feel better and exercise safely, for instance.
00:41:04 Speaker_01
I completely agree with that. I think there is a misconception out there about this, about what it means for something to be genetically heritable.
00:41:11 Speaker_01
And I think this gets to the root of why so many people find this sort of hard to believe that there's such a strong genetic components of body weight.
00:41:16 Speaker_01
And that's the idea that, you know, if you look at people, say 75 years ago, right, they were much leaner, right?
00:41:22 Speaker_01
And you look at people today and there's been this, starting sometime around, you know, the 1970s, there was this explosion in body weight and increase in obesity. And- Is that when, that's when it started, mid 70s?
00:41:30 Speaker_01
Sort of the 1970s is when a lot of that started happening.
00:41:32 Speaker_00
Snack food, snacking. So there's lots of explanations. Seed oil snack. By the way, I don't think that's the reason, folks. I think there are a lot of reasons, but the theories that abound right now on social media are,
00:41:44 Speaker_00
I have a list of theories as to why the obesity is increasing. You get everything from seed oils, to snacking, to smartphones, to conspiracies, to it's wild. It's wild. The range. of hypotheses as well.
00:41:59 Speaker_01
I mean, the challenges, I mean, some of them could be true, but it's just very hard to test those things experimentally, because they're happening in the whole population, right?
00:42:05 Speaker_01
But so I think the thing that people find hard to wrap their heads around, because it is a little bit of a confusing idea, is that how can it be that in say 50 or 75 years, there's been this explosion in obesity, which is the environment has changed, but human genetics has not changed in that amount of time.
00:42:19 Speaker_01
It's just not fast enough for people to evolve. So it can't be due to mutations in humans.
00:42:22 Speaker_00
What about Devolve? My understanding is that within a species, evolving new traits is very slow. But mutations arise, like the OB mutation, and then you can get very fat versions of an animal very quickly, right?
00:42:38 Speaker_00
All you need is a, you know, if it's a recessive allele, you need two copies, and the next thing you know, you've got a mouse that's four times larger than a typical mouse, and it's all explained by increased body weight.
00:42:49 Speaker_00
So that can happen very quickly within a species. what's rare to find is an entire new branch of a species that has a new adaptive function. That seems more rare.
00:43:02 Speaker_01
So that's true. So definitely there's some things that take longer to evolve than others, but with humans, we're talking about just two generations. There just isn't enough time for any evolution of any significance to happen.
00:43:11 Speaker_00
Baby boomers, right? Generic, that's me, right? And then whatever is YZ millennial, I lose track after that.
00:43:19 Speaker_01
So I think the thing that people find hard to wrap their heads around is how can it be that that increase in body weight is clearly environmental, right? Because that's all that's changed is the environment. Nothing has changed genetically.
00:43:29 Speaker_01
Yet it's also true what I said, that body weight is extremely heritable. It's one of the most heritable features and something on the order of 80%. One of the only things we know about that's actually more heritable than body weight is height, right?
00:43:39 Speaker_01
Most diseases are not as heritable as body weight. How can you explain that? And the idea is this, there's a distribution of body weights among people.
00:43:47 Speaker_01
So in any given society, at any point in time, some people are gonna be leaner, some people are gonna be more obese. That distribution, where you lie on that distribution, is determined primarily by genetics.
00:43:58 Speaker_01
So you may be the person who has the thrifty genes, so that basically cause you to save energy, and so you would be more on the obese side.
00:44:04 Speaker_01
Or you may be a person who has different genes that cause you to be a little bit less hungry, so you would be on the leaner side.
00:44:10 Speaker_01
what environment does is that it shifts that whole distribution so that basically the mean shifts so that everyone becomes, or most people become heavier.
00:44:19 Speaker_01
And so sort of a phrase that people sometimes use is that genetics loads the gun and environment pulls the trigger. So basically genetics sets your propensity and then environment can basically unmask that. And so
00:44:35 Speaker_01
as we've had this change in environment where there's all of this, and we don't know exactly what the things are that have changed that are important, but there's all this ultra-processed food, highly palatable food, just various other things that you mentioned, seed oils, who knows if that's important.
00:44:50 Speaker_01
Certain people had these latent mutations that made them, say, very sensitive to palatable food.
00:44:56 Speaker_01
In an earlier time, they may have been lean, but now because they have that latent capacity to be sensitive to ultra-processed food, they now gain tons of weight in the environment that we're in.
00:45:04 Speaker_01
It's still because of genetics, but it also requires the environmental component. I mean, just take a step back, right? You can make anyone lean by just putting them in prison and just only feeding them 1,500 calories.
00:45:15 Speaker_01
I mean, we've done those kinds of experiments. There's this famous experiment, the Minnesota starvation experiment, right? They basically put people in prison, but this is in World War II.
00:45:23 Speaker_01
took a bunch of healthy volunteers, fed them 1600 calories a day, and just asked what would happen if you basically semi-starved people, and unsurprisingly, they lose an incredible amount of weight, all they think about is food, basically their body temperature goes down, their heart rate goes down, they just become obsessed with food.
00:45:36 Speaker_01
And you could always do that for anyone, right? But in a given environment where you're not in that kind of situation, then your propensity to gain weight will be determined by genetics. So that's the idea.
00:45:46 Speaker_00
I very much appreciate that description, and I know a great number of other people will as well,
00:45:54 Speaker_00
the explanation for the increase in obesity has not been described with that level of accuracy and detail with respect to the interactions between genetics and the environment.
00:46:05 Speaker_00
Is it fair to say that what's changed in our environment is the free availability of food? You know, I was walking through an airport yesterday and every 20 meters or so there's a vending machine or a restaurant. The cost of calories is fairly low.
00:46:22 Speaker_00
Right, getting high quality nutritious food that tastes great is expensive, I would argue. But getting calories is fairly inexpensive.
00:46:33 Speaker_01
I think that's a plausible hypothesis. It's one of several plausible hypotheses and it would be surprising to me if it didn't contribute.
00:46:41 Speaker_01
But the reality is these population level questions, it's just so hard to actually know because you can't do an experiment, right? We can't create a parallel society where we manipulate one of these variables and see if the people become obese.
00:46:51 Speaker_01
So I think probably the availability of food, the free availability, the low cost is one part of it.
00:46:57 Speaker_01
Another part of it is probably, although again it's not proven, is that these ultra-processed foods have a number of features that make people prone to gain weight.
00:47:08 Speaker_01
And there's really beautiful work, I don't know if you know this, from Kevin Hall at the NIH who's investigated this. really, in my opinion, the best person doing this kind of human obesity research today.
00:47:16 Speaker_01
And he does these experiments where he takes people into the NIH, into the hospital, hospitalizes them for several weeks so he can exactly control what they eat.
00:47:24 Speaker_01
And he did this beautiful experiment where basically he had chefs prepare two kinds of food, one ultra-processed and the other not ultra-processed, sort of more whole foods, more healthier foods.
00:47:34 Speaker_01
but had them take a lot of care so that when they gave the foods to independent raters, to people to test, they would say, this is about equally palatable. So I like this ultra-processed dish as much as this non-ultra-processed dish.
00:47:46 Speaker_00
What's an example of an ultra-processed dish? Like a out-of-package macaroni and cheese? Exactly, that kind of stuff. With bacon kind of thing? Exactly. Versus some pasta sitting next to a vegetable and some- Exactly. A nice piece of salmon or something?
00:47:58 Speaker_00
Exactly, exactly.
00:48:01 Speaker_01
and took people into the hospital, basically allowed them to eat just as much as they would like, first of the ultra-processed meals, so they had the selection of ultra-processed meals for a couple weeks, and then switched them to the non-ultra-processed meals, and then also did it in the reverse order, so the other half of the people, they got the regular food first, then they got the ultra-processed food.
00:48:21 Speaker_01
And what he found is that even though people rated the foods as equally palatable, they ate much more of the ultra-processed food. And they actually gained weight during that two week period when they were being given the ultra-processed foods.
00:48:32 Speaker_01
And then when you switched them, they lost weight. So the idea being that you can have two sets of food that you have equal preferences for, but something about the ultra-processed food is making you eat more of it when you actually consume it.
00:48:44 Speaker_01
And there's a number of ideas about why that could be. So one idea is that these ultra-processed foods have been optimized to have the right percentage of fat and sugar and protein to sort of promote more consumption once you start eating it.
00:48:56 Speaker_01
So that could be part of it. Another idea is that, you know, a big thing about whole foods is that they take more energy to digest and they have more volume.
00:49:04 Speaker_01
So one of the striking things from that study is if you just look at the pictures of the meals, they're the same number of calories, but there's so much more food seemingly on the non-processed food versus the ultra processed food.
00:49:14 Speaker_01
And that's just because whole foods are bigger because they're not so energy dense. So, and we know that for example, volume is a major signal on the short term for regulating food intake. So if you just eat more volume, that could be valuable.
00:49:27 Speaker_01
And there's lots of things like that. So I think that's another plausible hypothesis, but the truth is we don't really know.
00:49:32 Speaker_00
I have a hypothesis and I don't want to force you into speculation, but given that you've studied and discovered that the neurons and circuits involved in appetitive and consummatory behaviors can learn based on experience and expectation,
00:49:49 Speaker_00
I think it's fair game to at least ask your thoughts on this.
00:49:52 Speaker_00
So I've been paying a lot of attention to the landscape of what the general public think about, let's call them elimination diets, where people will just eat meat, or will go into a vegan diet, or do some time restricted feeding, or do any number of different things that have been shown to promote weight loss.
00:50:14 Speaker_00
provided people obey the laws of thermodynamics and consume fewer calories than they burn, right? I do believe in calories in, calories out.
00:50:23 Speaker_00
And there are a number of different routes to get there, and some are more painful, some are less painful, and it depends on the individual lifestyle, exercise, and on and on.
00:50:31 Speaker_00
But let's just suppose for a moment, based on Kevin's work on highly processed foods versus whole foods, that there's a learning that takes place when we eat,
00:50:44 Speaker_00
and that this learning takes place over time such that our brain and appetite start to link the variables of taste, macronutrients, proteins, fats, and carbohydrates, sort of knowledge about macronutrients.
00:50:57 Speaker_00
A piece of fish is mostly protein, has some fat. A bowl of rice is mostly carbohydrate, has some protein. Put a pat of butter on it, has some fat also, right? It's sort of obvious.
00:51:12 Speaker_00
Taste, macronutrient content, calories, which we already know people with anorexia are exquisitely good at counting with their eyes. So it's possible they represent, again, a pathologic extreme of this.
00:51:28 Speaker_00
And micronutrient content, maybe even amino acid content, like how much leucine is there. Now, most people aren't thinking about how much leucine is in a meal, but we know that Leucine is important for certain aspects of muscle metabolism.
00:51:42 Speaker_00
It's present in certain proteins and not others. You're going to find less of it in a vegetable typically than you would in a piece of chicken and so on. And that when people eat mostly non-processed or minimally processed foods and not in combination.
00:51:58 Speaker_00
So we're not talking about stewing all this together or blending all of it together, which sounds disgusting, right?
00:52:04 Speaker_00
Broccoli rice and a chicken breast blend together just sounds horrible, but eating them separately, if there's some olive oil and a little pat of butter involved, like that sounds pretty good.
00:52:11 Speaker_00
but a highly processed food in some ways is a blending together of macronutrients, micronutrients, if there are any, and other features of the food that neurons in the brain seem to pay attention to, and then giving it a unified taste, a Dorito, right?
00:52:30 Speaker_00
A candy bar that we attach to the product, we attach to the name of the processed food, to the packaging,
00:52:37 Speaker_00
But I could imagine, and here's the hypothesis, that that is quote unquote confusing to our neural circuits in a way that doesn't match up well with our thermodynamic requirements of how much we're burning versus how much we need to eat.
00:52:52 Speaker_00
Whereas when I eat a piece of steak and a vegetable, I actually want less carbohydrate afterwards. If I eat the carbohydrate first, for me, it's difficult, because I love the taste of carbohydrates, especially when they're combined with fat.
00:53:05 Speaker_00
But there seems to be an easier time regulating food intake when people step back and say, I'm going to consume minimally processed whole foods.
00:53:14 Speaker_00
And I'm guessing it's not just because they're trying to be healthier, that might be what stimulates the shift, but that the brain starts to learn the relationship between food volume,
00:53:26 Speaker_00
smell, taste, what these things look like and satiation at the level of, oh, that's enough amino acids because I had a piece of fish.
00:53:33 Speaker_00
So maybe I don't need to consume as much of some other things or the vegetables provide volume and fiber and often vegetables can taste really delicious too.
00:53:41 Speaker_00
So that there's a linking of nutrients, calories and taste in a way that's more appropriately matched to the energetic demands of the organism, in this case, us humans. that highly processed foods bypass.
00:53:56 Speaker_00
Okay, now I realized that was long-winded and forgive me. but my audience is used to that.
00:54:01 Speaker_00
Whenever I'm trying to table something for, no pun intended, for discussion that I would like to think can at least stimulate some additional thinking about a landscape, in this case, nutrition and feeding behavior, that for a lot of people is just really confusing.
00:54:16 Speaker_00
And here's why, and this is the last thing I'll say. I have several friends who have been very overweight their entire lives, for whom the following diet has worked exceptionally well. I'm not a diet coach, I'm not a nutritionist.
00:54:30 Speaker_00
I don't pretend to be one. I say eat proteins like meat, fish, eggs, vegetables, and fruit, and do that for a couple of months, and then add back in starches as you see fit based on your food intake. And without fail, they all lose a ton of weight.
00:54:47 Speaker_00
They're very happy with that. They add back in a minimum of starches. They keep the weight off and they're also exercising, but not more than they were before in most cases.
00:54:59 Speaker_00
And I don't think that it's meat or fish or vegetables per se, I think it's that they finally... develop an appreciation for what different foods have in terms of what they actually need.
00:55:09 Speaker_00
And without fail, they all say, oh, you know, I went to this party and I had a piece of cake and it didn't taste good to me after three or four bites. So that's interesting too. So I just would like your thoughts on this.
00:55:19 Speaker_00
We're not defining any new diets. I don't sell any diets. I don't do any of that.
00:55:23 Speaker_00
But I find it amazing that when people start eating minimally processed whole foods, I have to assume that their brain changes as it relates to appetite, craving, and just kind of an unconscious understanding about what food is providing them or not.
00:55:37 Speaker_00
And that highly processed foods basically bypass all of this and just get you to consume more. Perhaps in hopes of getting something that you probably aren't getting at all or that you need to consume a lot of this food in order to get.
00:55:52 Speaker_01
There's several interesting ideas there. So there's two that come to mind, just thinking about what you just said. So the one is the idea of what's going on when these people consume simpler diets, more of a whole foods.
00:56:03 Speaker_01
And one thing I think that's very likely going on, is this phenomenon of sensory-specific satiety as being engaged.
00:56:12 Speaker_01
And so sensory-specific satiety is just the idea that as you expose yourself repeatedly to a certain flavor or taste, you basically lose appetite for that. You get specific loss of appetite for that flavor or taste.
00:56:25 Speaker_01
This is why, as you said, basically if you start off eating the protein, after a while you're like, I don't want any more salmon, but I would like some carbohydrates now, because you have this sensory-specific satiety.
00:56:34 Speaker_01
And so it's well known actually that if you simplify your diet, make your diet really simple so there's just a few things, the sensory specific satiety alone can cause you to eat less.
00:56:44 Speaker_01
Basically because there's just less variety in your diet and you don't want to eat more of that same thing. And so I think a lot of diets actually
00:56:51 Speaker_01
It's not about the specific macronutrient or the specific food, it's just that they're reducing the variety in the diet. Eventually you just get sick of eating the same thing.
00:57:00 Speaker_01
And the thought behind that idea is that it's important evolutionarily so that you eat a diverse diet. It's the reason probably that you want sweets after you've eaten a savory meal and so on.
00:57:09 Speaker_01
A second idea though that comes to mind is just, as you mentioned, this idea of learning. And so much about our preferences for food are, they're not innate, they're driven by learning. So there are some things that are innate.
00:57:22 Speaker_01
So if you put sugar on a baby's tongue, it'll smile, indicating that it likes it. And if you put something bitter, it'll frown. And a rat will do the same thing, a neonate rat. But most of flavor and the perception of food is not just sweet or bitter.
00:57:38 Speaker_01
It's this much more complex sensation that involves smells, it involves tastes, and then it involves how those tastes and smells interact with the post-ingestive effects of the nutrients.
00:57:49 Speaker_01
So the sensing of those nutrients in your stomach and in your intestine, primarily in your intestine, are thought to then feed back and then change your preference for these foods. And so, you know,
00:57:59 Speaker_01
There's lots of examples of this that you can just imagine from everyday experience. Most people, the first time they had a beer, or the first time they had a glass of coffee, found it repulsive, right? Because it's extremely bitter.
00:58:08 Speaker_01
But then we come to crave these things because we know what they do to our body. We like what they do to our body. And that doesn't just make us take them like they're medicine. We actually somehow change our very perception of how that flavor is.
00:58:17 Speaker_01
We actually come to savor that flavor we previously found disgusting. And it's because our sensation of whether something's good or bad depends on our internal state.
00:58:26 Speaker_01
And so it's an interesting idea, you know, perhaps if these ultra processed foods that have so many different ingredients and such an unnatural combination,
00:58:34 Speaker_01
perhaps this process of learning about the nutrient content of different foods and flavors becomes impaired because it's just the brain is not used, the brain's used to saying, you know, this is a piece of chicken and this is primarily protein.
00:58:47 Speaker_01
And so I can gauge, you know, from this flavor, I can connect this flavor to an amino acid content, but something that's so diverse, it might be harder to do.
00:58:55 Speaker_00
And isn't it the case that the neurons in the gut and the hormones that are produced by the gut
00:59:02 Speaker_00
as we digest food and that the neurons in the brain that control appetite and feeding have to be tuned to macronutrient content because those are the primary colors of nutrients and nutrients are the way in which we can persist on a day-to-day basis, right?
00:59:22 Speaker_00
I mean, I'm not trying to sound more sophisticated where simpler terms would suffice. What I'm basically saying is that the neurons in our brains that control these behaviors
00:59:31 Speaker_00
both eating and cessation of eating an ingredient or an entire meal can't be tuned to a particular food product or to chicken or to an egg or to a steak or to lentils, but rather to amino acid content, essential amino acid content in particular, essential fatty acids.
00:59:50 Speaker_00
And in the case of carbohydrate, whatever is going to replace whatever glycogen we might've depleted, right? I mean, like if we really break it down into biology, eating is for a purpose.
00:59:59 Speaker_00
And my understanding is that the purpose of eating is to replace those things as needed rather than to, you know, taste savory or taste, you know.
01:00:11 Speaker_01
Absolutely, absolutely. Those are just, those things, those sensory cues are just markers that tell the brain what might be in that substance. I think if you look at broadly at this difference between calories and macronutrients and micronutrients,
01:00:24 Speaker_01
I would say what you see is that most of the circuits that are controlling hunger are primarily calorie specific.
01:00:30 Speaker_01
So they can, like for example, an AGRP neuron, I can put sugar, fat, or protein into the stomach of a mouse and to an equal extent inhibit an AGRP neuron as long as they have equal calories.
01:00:41 Speaker_00
Yeah. So a little drop of olive oil into the belly that has of an animal that has, let's drop, let's say a little bit more, let's say 120 calories of olive oil. is equal potent to 120 calories of chicken breast?
01:00:59 Speaker_01
At the level of these AGRP neurons, it is. So they don't care about the macronutrients? No, they're really concerned about energy.
01:01:09 Speaker_01
There are circuitries that are more concerned with macronutrients individually, although I don't think we know nearly as much about how that works. And I think the evidence is clear that the strongest defended macronutrient by far is protein.
01:01:21 Speaker_01
So protein, I don't think really sugar and fat intake are strongly defended in the sense that you're fine if you go without eating sugar, right? Basically, you can synthesize sugar from amino acids, for example.
01:01:34 Speaker_01
And you don't develop a specific sugar appetite in the same way you do, for example, if you deprive yourself of hunger and you develop a protein hunger. I think the difference is that proteins consist of essential amino acids.
01:01:46 Speaker_01
I forget if it's nine amino acids that your body cannot synthesize. You absolutely need them or you will die. Whereas sugar and fat can be interchanged with other macronutrients.
01:01:58 Speaker_01
So, and there's other things also that you absolutely need to ingest like sodium chloride, right?
01:02:03 Speaker_01
So sodium, so there's very few deprive an animal of sodium, they will develop this salt appetite that's incredible basically, and that's completely innate.
01:02:13 Speaker_01
But that's, I think salt appetite and protein appetite are the things that are probably the most strongly regulated at the level of the macro and micronutrients.
01:02:23 Speaker_00
I'd like to take a brief break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need.
01:02:29 Speaker_00
That means the electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium in the correct amounts and ratios and nothing you don't, which means no sugar.
01:02:36 Speaker_00
Now, I and others on this podcast have talked about the critical importance of hydration for proper brain and body functioning. even a slight degree of dehydration can diminish cognitive and physical performance.
01:02:46 Speaker_00
It's also important that you get adequate electrolytes. The electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium, are critical for the functioning of all the cells in your body, especially your neurons, your nerve cells.
01:02:57 Speaker_00
Drinking element dissolved in water makes it very easy to ensure that you're getting adequate hydration and adequate electrolytes.
01:03:03 Speaker_00
To make sure I'm getting proper amounts of hydration and electrolytes, I dissolve one packet of Element in about 16 to 32 ounces of water when I wake up in the morning, and I drink that basically first thing in the morning.
01:03:13 Speaker_00
I'll also drink Element dissolved in water during any kind of physical exercise I'm doing, especially in hot days when I'm sweating a lot, losing water and electrolytes. They have a bunch of different great-tasting flavors of Element.
01:03:25 Speaker_00
My favorite is the watermelon, although I confess I also like the raspberry and the citrus. Basically, I like all the flavors. And Element has also just released a new line of canned sparkling Element.
01:03:34 Speaker_00
So these aren't the packets you dissolve in water, these are cans of Element that you crack open like any other canned drink, like a soda. but you're getting your hydration and your electrolytes with no sugar.
01:03:44 Speaker_00
If you'd like to try Element, you can go to drinkelement spelled L-M-N-T dot com slash Huberman to claim a free Element sample pack with the purchase of any Element drink mix. Again, that's drinkelement.com slash Huberman to claim a free sample pack.
01:03:59 Speaker_00
If we could talk about body weight homeostasis for a moment that I think that would be useful. So let's say somebody decides they want to lose some weight. caloric restricts slightly either by exercising more or eating less or both.
01:04:15 Speaker_00
Their body weight drops by a bit. Let's say they lose 10 pounds, eight of which are body fat. They lose a little bit of lean mass also. They're now at a new lower body weight. Are the AGRP neurons motivated to have them seek out more food?
01:04:29 Speaker_00
In other words, are they hungrier and more motivated to find and eat food? Or do these AGRP neurons learn, hey, body weight is lower and I don't need to push to find so much food so often?
01:04:42 Speaker_01
No, I mean, the idea is that the AGRP neurons are more active when you lose weight and that chronic activation of those neurons, in part because leptin levels are lower in the blood because you've lost weight,
01:04:52 Speaker_01
is that drive, that counter-regulatory drive that drives you to then consume more food. But then how do people ever keep weight off? Well, so part of the answer is they don't. I mean, so there's so- Really?
01:05:05 Speaker_00
I would argue like I have these friends who were very heavy. Most of the excess weight was body fat for a long time. They seem to be doing great eating the way that I described before. And by the way, I'm not a proponent of any one particular diet.
01:05:19 Speaker_00
I have vegan friends, carnivore friends, et cetera. But that pattern of eating I described before has been enormously successful for them. I haven't run a, you know, a randomized controlled trial.
01:05:27 Speaker_00
It's not my job to do that in the realm of nutrition, but they're doing great. They claim to be sated. They are so happy with the way things are going. And I don't hear that they're constantly hungry. I hear that they're constantly sated.
01:05:41 Speaker_01
Well, so I would say that, you know, There have been efforts for a long time to develop diets that would help people consistently lose weight. and it has been very unsuccessful.
01:05:53 Speaker_01
There are some people who for various reasons can successfully lose weight and keep it off.
01:05:57 Speaker_01
And I don't know that I have a good answer for what's going on in those individual cases, how they are the exceptions to the rule, what about them is different that makes sense.
01:06:04 Speaker_00
Some also quit drinking alcohol. Yeah, so there's other things.
01:06:06 Speaker_01
So, you know, I think- So behavioral regulation is better when you're sober as opposed to- You can change your environment. But you know, so what this is sort of getting at is what is the counter-regulatory response to weight loss?
01:06:18 Speaker_01
And so this has been studied. It was first studied in the context of energy expenditure. And because energy expenditure is actually surprisingly easier to measure in humans than food intake, because people don't tell you accurately what food they eat.
01:06:31 Speaker_01
If they're free living humans, they have to fill out a questionnaire. And the idea is that for every kilogram of weight you lose, so about 2.2 pounds, I think, your energy expenditure decreases by about 30 kilocalories a day.
01:06:48 Speaker_01
Now, so not a ton, but that is significant, right? 30 calories. And then if you lose, as you said, 10 pounds, then that's 150 calories. And that adds up over time.
01:06:59 Speaker_01
One interesting thing about that is that if you take people who were obese and then they've lost a ton of weight. So there's a study by Rudy Liebel about 25 years ago that did this.
01:07:08 Speaker_01
Take people who lost like a hundred pounds and then take a control group that has the same height, weight, basically the same body composition as those people who've now lost 100 pounds. Compare their energy expenditure.
01:07:21 Speaker_01
The energy expenditure in the people that lost all the weight is about 25% lower than the people who never were obese. And so those people who lost the weight, we call them the reduced obese, so that's what they were called in those studies.
01:07:33 Speaker_01
And the idea is that there's now this chronic deficit, they have to eat 25% less than someone who looks the same as them, is the same height as them, same weight as them, in order to maintain that body weight.
01:07:43 Speaker_01
What's unclear is whether that's because those people simply always had a slower metabolism, they were always destined to be obese, and then you're just basically you're comparing two different groups, or whether something about the process of gaining weight and being in a higher weight for a longer period of time changes the brain, so that then once you lose the weight, it's irreversible.
01:07:59 Speaker_01
But there have been studies looking at at least a year and it doesn't seem to come back within a year, that difference in energy expenditure. Now the question is, is that really the big effect?
01:08:08 Speaker_01
Is that why it's so hard to lose weight, energy expenditure, or is it because you're hungrier? And that's actually much harder to measure. But there was another really nice study, again by Kevin Hall, investigating this.
01:08:19 Speaker_01
He used a really clever approach, this drug. So basically what he wanted to do was, is he reasoned that you can measure people's body weight, and you can measure people's energy expenditure.
01:08:30 Speaker_01
And because calories in, calories out, if we can measure body weight and energy expenditure accurately, we can then back calculate how much that person was actually eating. So let's see what happens when you have people lose weight.
01:08:43 Speaker_01
How does their food intake change? But the trick to this is you need to do it in such a way that you don't just tell them to go run on a treadmill.
01:08:50 Speaker_01
Because if you tell someone to go run on a treadmill and lose weight, then basically they're thinking about the fact that they're doing this.
01:08:54 Speaker_01
So you need to do it in some way covertly so that you increase their energy expenditure, cause them to lose weight, but without them realizing that's what's happening.
01:09:00 Speaker_01
So they gave them these drugs, these SGLT2 inhibitors, and it's a pill you can take. They're used for diabetes. They block this protein, SGLT2, in the kidney that is necessary for glucose to be reabsorbed into the blood.
01:09:14 Speaker_01
And so basically what happens is you pee out about 90 grams of glucose a day. But you don't know that you're doing that. And that causes you to lose energy. And these people would lose some weight. And then measure how their food intake changes.
01:09:26 Speaker_01
And what that showed is that for every two pounds or so of weight you lose, your hunger goes up by 100 calories per day.
01:09:33 Speaker_01
So basically, you've got a 30 kilocalorie decrease in energy expenditure, 100 kilocalorie decrease in appetite for every two pounds you lose, on average. Some people will be exceptions, right?
01:09:41 Speaker_01
And they won't experience that at all for aspects of their physiology we don't understand. And so the increased hunger seems to be the main reason people find it so difficult to keep weight off.
01:09:53 Speaker_00
GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide one, Ozempic, Monjaro, and similar drugs.
01:10:00 Speaker_00
My understanding of the back history on these is that a biologist obsessed with Gila monsters, a reptile that doesn't need to eat very often discovered a peptide within their bloodstream called Xtendin that allowed them to eat very seldom.
01:10:20 Speaker_00
It curbed appetite in the Gila monster of all things. And it has a analog homolog, you know, we don't know. I don't know the sequence homology exactly, but there's a similar peptide made in mice and in humans that suppresses appetite.
01:10:39 Speaker_00
If you would, could you tell us what is known about how GLP-1 works to suppress appetite where in the body and or brain? And your sort of read of these drugs and what's happening there, good, bad, exciting, ugly.
01:10:57 Speaker_01
Anything else? So the story of GLP-1, so the Gila monster is an important turn. And I'll talk about that. It actually goes back before that quite a ways. So I should take a step back and say, you know, these were developed as drugs for diabetes, right?
01:11:09 Speaker_01
And so, and diabetes is a condition where basically you have elevated blood glucose, either because you don't produce enough insulin or because your insulin is not effective.
01:11:19 Speaker_01
And so back in sort of the 1920s, right around the time insulin was discovered, there was this phenomenon discovered known as the incretin effect. And what it was- Incretin? Incretin, yeah. Not the cretin effect. Not the cretin effect.
01:11:32 Speaker_00
You can observe the cretin effect in numerous places in daily life and online, just kidding.
01:11:37 Speaker_01
So it's called the incretin effect. You can think of it as increase insulin, because that's what the effect is.
01:11:43 Speaker_01
And the idea was that if you take glucose by mouth, if you consume glucose orally versus if you have the same amount of glucose injected intravenously, more insulin is produced when you take the glucose orally versus if it's delivered intravenously.
01:11:58 Speaker_01
Suggesting something about the process of ingesting the glucose causes more insulin to be released and causes you to lower your body sugar more accurately and more strongly.
01:12:08 Speaker_00
Interesting.
01:12:09 Speaker_01
Which is a little bit counterintuitive because In the pancreas, right, so insulin is released from the pancreas, from the beta cell. the pancreas senses the glucose concentration in the blood directly.
01:12:17 Speaker_01
And so it suggests that insulin is being released, not just in response to changes in blood glucose, but in response to a second factor. And so they call that an incretin.
01:12:25 Speaker_01
And through various experiments, it was shown that this incretin effect comes from the intestine, that there's some substance being produced by the intestine that when you eat a meal, sugar goes through your intestine, that boosts this insulin response to glucose in the blood.
01:12:42 Speaker_01
And people immediately realized this could potentially be very valuable. And the reason is that, you know, you can treat diabetes with insulin injections, but insulin is dangerous, right?
01:12:49 Speaker_01
Because if you inject too much insulin, you can kill yourself by making yourself hypoglycemic, right? So you have to be very careful.
01:12:54 Speaker_01
But the thing about the increase in effect is, it's not causing insulin release directly, but it's rather boosting the natural insulin release that comes when your glucose is higher in your blood.
01:13:03 Speaker_01
So it's sort of an amplifier on the natural insulin release. So basically in the years that followed, whenever someone would find a new hormone, they would test it, is it this incretin? And there was lots of failures, they weren't the incretin.
01:13:15 Speaker_01
But then, so there's this other hormone that comes from the pancreas called glucagon. And so glucagon, it was also discovered in the 1920s, glucagon is kind of the anti-insulin.
01:13:25 Speaker_01
So when blood sugar goes low, glucagon is released in order to cause your liver to release glucose into the blood. So glucagon and insulin are these two opposing hormones.
01:13:35 Speaker_01
Glucagon was known for a long time, but people discovered in the 1980s that the glucagon gene is expressed in other tissues other than the pancreas, and it's differentially processed.
01:13:46 Speaker_01
The protein is differentially processed to produce different hormones, hormones other than glucagon.
01:13:51 Speaker_01
And they discovered there was one in the intestine, and so they called it glucagon-like peptide because it comes from the same gene, but it's just slightly different. It's cut up slightly differently. And this hormone wasn't in creatine.
01:14:01 Speaker_01
So basically, if you put it on beta cells, you get this increased response of insulin in response to glucose. And so there was the idea, OK, this could be a great diabetes drug. And I should say, there is one other in creatine that's been found.
01:14:15 Speaker_01
It's called GIP, G-I-P. And that will be important in talking about some of these other drugs. Also, a hormone that comes from the intestine. And so the challenge with making GLP-1 into a drug is that it has an extremely short half-life.
01:14:30 Speaker_01
So it has a half-life of about two minutes in the blood. And so even if you inject people with GLP-1, it won't really be useful for anything. You don't decrease appetite, you don't affect blood sugar, it's just degraded too fast.
01:14:42 Speaker_01
And the reason it's degraded is because there's an enzyme, DPP-4 is what it's called, that degrades GLP-1. So the first thing people tried was, let's make inhibitors of that enzyme so we can boost this natural GLP-1 signal.
01:14:53 Speaker_01
And those are approved diabetes drugs. They're called gliptins. You've probably heard about them. Genuvia is the most common one. And those boost the level of GLP-1, the natural GLP-1 produced from the intestine by about threefold.
01:15:07 Speaker_01
And they're effective in treating diabetes. Not as effective- People lose weight. People do not lose weight.
01:15:12 Speaker_00
Interesting.
01:15:12 Speaker_01
And that's one of the key reasons that we know the natural function of GLP-1 is not really to control body weight because you can boost the level threefold with these DPP-4 drugs. Millions of people have taken them. They do not lose weight.
01:15:24 Speaker_01
That's a great question. So, but, you know, a threefold is great, but like you'd like to increase it even more, right? And to do that, you can't block this enzyme. You have to actually produce a GLP-1 that is more stable in the blood.
01:15:36 Speaker_01
And that's where this lizard that you mentioned comes into play. It produces a stabilized form of GLP-1 and it's a venom. No one knows why.
01:15:45 Speaker_01
One hypothesis is that it's something to do with the lizard, as you said, basically having this long time period between meals and it needs to regulate its blood glucose. Who knows if that is true?
01:15:54 Speaker_01
but it turned out to be fortuitous because then this GLP-1 from this lizard, it has a half-life of like two hours. And so the first GLP-1 drug that was approved was just this molecule from this lizard, basically.
01:16:06 Speaker_01
And it's called Xenotide, and it was approved in 2005. Works well for diabetes, has a half-life of two hours. You inject it, and it doesn't cause a ton of weight loss. But two hours is good, but it's not so great.
01:16:22 Speaker_01
So then the pharmaceutical industry said, can we basically improve this even further? And so they start engineering this hormone, making mutations, attaching lipid tails to make it bind to proteins in the blood that would stabilize it.
01:16:34 Speaker_01
Chemistry jockey stuff. Yeah, exactly. And I think the next big advance was this compound liraglutide. And liraglutide was approved for diabetes in 2010 and then for weight loss in 2014. And so liraglutide has a half-life of about 13 hours in the blood.
01:16:51 Speaker_01
Now you're getting up to something serious. We've gone from two minutes, two hours, 13 hours. And you get better effects on aspects of blood glucose and diabetes control. And they started to see that some people were losing weight.
01:17:04 Speaker_01
Very variable responses, not everyone loses weight on liraglutide.
01:17:08 Speaker_01
And one of the things they noticed that I think is just as fascinating, just sort of example of how drug discovery works in the real world, a lot of these people who take liraglutide, now it has this longer half-life, they start to get nauseous.
01:17:18 Speaker_01
And that would limit how much of the liraglutide they could take. And it's a known side effect of these GLP-1 drugs, it causes nausea and sort of this gastrointestinal distress. But they noticed that over time, the nausea would just sort of go away.
01:17:29 Speaker_01
And so they would start dose escalating, sort of raising the dose that the person would take.
01:17:33 Speaker_01
So you would go, you know, a month at this dose, and then a month at a slightly higher dose, and then a month at a slightly higher dose, and you could work your way up. And these side effects would reappear, but then they'd go away.
01:17:44 Speaker_01
And then once you got up to the highest doses, then people really started losing weight. And so there's a couple of things that our pharmaceutical industry realized, wow, these are potentially really effective weight loss drugs.
01:17:52 Speaker_01
And also this nausea, which we thought was a killer, people are able to just get used to it and then it just goes away. It undergoes, the word is tachyphylaxis.
01:18:01 Speaker_01
So the idea is that the receptor that's affecting, in the gut that's causing these effects, it undergoes some sort of down regulation with chronic exposure.
01:18:12 Speaker_01
So liraglutide, you know, it's been around, you know, it's been on the market for 14 years now, was used, but still you're only getting sort of like seven to 10% weight loss, which is good, but not like, you know, amazing, impressive.
01:18:26 Speaker_01
But then semaglutide came along, and that was approved for diabetes in 2017. And semaglutide is ozempic, or also marketed as Wigovi for weight loss. And semaglutide now has a half-life of seven days.
01:18:41 Speaker_01
So now we've gone from two minutes, two hours, 13 hours, seven days. And you can really jack up the concentration with a seven-day half-life. And then they saw people start really losing weight.
01:18:53 Speaker_01
And so in some of those trials, people lost 16% of their body weight, which previously had been unattainable. In what timeframe? Typically takes about a year.
01:19:03 Speaker_00
And most of the loss in body weight is from body fat or from other compartments?
01:19:09 Speaker_01
The typical number is that if you lose weight either through dieting or through taking one of these drugs and you don't do anything like eat a high protein diet or do resistance training, somewhere between 25 and 33% of what you lose is going to be muscle.
01:19:22 Speaker_01
The rest is going to be fat.
01:19:23 Speaker_00
But as you said, some of that could be offset by resistance training and or consuming a higher protein diet.
01:19:30 Speaker_01
Yeah, you can almost completely eliminate that if you eat enough protein and do serious weightlifting. Obviously not the whole population is interested in doing that. And there's been a lot of discussion of how serious a side effect this is.
01:19:42 Speaker_01
Among elderly people, you don't want to be losing muscle mass because you're already losing so much muscle mass. On the other hand, the counter argument that has been made, which I think is also kind of convincing, is that
01:19:53 Speaker_01
True, you're losing some muscle, but you're also losing all this fat and you no longer need as much muscle when you're not carrying around as much body fat.
01:20:01 Speaker_01
So people who are heavier naturally have more muscle because they need to, to move their body, right?
01:20:04 Speaker_00
And so- Yeah, the calves on very obese people are often enormous and then they lose weight. Exactly. And I mentioned the calves in particular because they're carrying a lot of the body load.
01:20:18 Speaker_01
Exactly, exactly. So it's still an open question as to how serious a problem this lean muscle mass loss is. Although the pharmaceutical industry is all in now on making drugs that basically are gonna prevent that.
01:20:29 Speaker_01
So that's something that will be happening probably in the future.
01:20:32 Speaker_00
Is it a, sorry to interrupt, but is the weight loss on these drugs the consequence of reduced appetite?
01:20:39 Speaker_00
or some other aspect of metabolism, and if it's the consequence of reduced appetite, is that occurring at the level of the brain and gut, or a combination?
01:20:50 Speaker_01
So it's almost entirely reduced appetite, and it's almost entirely occurring at the level of the brain. Which neurons? It's thought that the key targets of these drugs are neurons in these two regions, one's called the nucleus of the solitary tract,
01:21:03 Speaker_01
And the other one's called the areopostrema. So we're back in the brainstem. Back in the brainstem. So these are actually the neurons in that decerebrate rat story I was telling earlier.
01:21:12 Speaker_01
These are the brain regions that are preserved in the decerebrate rat. The decerebrate rat still has these very caudal brainstem structures. They're two very special brain regions because they get direct input from the vagus nerve.
01:21:23 Speaker_01
So the vagus nerve is the nerve that innervates your stomach and intestines and heart and lungs.
01:21:28 Speaker_01
And it's sort of the major pathway from gut to brain and provides most of the sensor of the neural input from gut to brain telling you about things like this, your stomach distention, how many nutrients are in your intestine, breathing, all that stuff.
01:21:40 Speaker_01
And almost all of those vagal nerves terminate on these two structures in the brainstem.
01:21:45 Speaker_00
When I hear post-trauma, I think about nausea because I was taught that post-trauma contains neurons that can stimulate vomiting. And this seems to link up well, at least in the logical sense with the idea that
01:21:58 Speaker_00
stimulating, activating receptors in these neurons within post-tremor might explain part of the transient nausea side effect of ozempic and related drugs.
01:22:07 Speaker_01
Yeah, so the current thought is that a lot of the nausea is coming from activating the neurons of the area post-tremor, and that a lot of the sort of physiologic satiety is coming from activating the neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract.
01:22:18 Speaker_01
Now, the whole brain is connected to each other, and so if you really turn on these neurons in the NTS and the AP, they're gonna talk to the hypothalamus and all these other brain regions, it's gonna change the whole brain.
01:22:26 Speaker_01
So it's not just those regions, you know, these drugs don't have great access to the brain. They can penetrate a little bit into the brain, but they don't penetrate into the whole brain.
01:22:34 Speaker_01
And it's thought that if you take fluorescently labeled versions of these drugs and see where do they, so you can visualize where do they actually go, they're enriched in these structures in the brainstem.
01:22:43 Speaker_01
So that's why people think that this is probably where they're acting.
01:22:46 Speaker_00
And is that because there's an abundance of the receptors for the, these compounds in post-trauma and NTS, or is it because the blood-brain barrier is somehow weaker at that location?
01:22:59 Speaker_01
It's because the blood-brain barrier is weaker. So basically it's a region, what's known as a circumventricular organ, meaning it's one of these rare places in the brain where the blood-brain barrier is weakened.
01:23:09 Speaker_01
And so substances can come from the outside into the brain. And that's important for these big peptides, because these are not small molecules. These are big peptides with lipid chains on them and other things.
01:23:20 Speaker_01
that can really only get into areas of the brain where the blood-brain barrier is weakened.
01:23:24 Speaker_00
I really appreciate that you mentioned the half-life issue with GLP-1 and the fact that these DPP-4 antagonists did not lead to weight loss despite increasing circulating GLP by threefold.
01:23:40 Speaker_00
This is relevant to a number of different claims that people make that a given food or a given drink increases GLP-1. I've actually said before, you know, I'm a big consumer of yerba mate,
01:23:53 Speaker_00
father's side is Argentine and it's a known appetite suppressant, but it contains caffeine and other stimulants that might explain some of that.
01:24:00 Speaker_00
And it's not a robust appetite suppressant to the point where most people would rely on it as a weight loss compound. But anyway, it's my preferred source of caffeine, but I've said before.
01:24:11 Speaker_00
there's some evidence that it can increase GLP-1, but based on what you've said, the increases in GLP-1 that it creates are very unlikely to produce the kind of appetite suppressive effect that would lead to any significant weight loss in somebody that's obese, presumably that are separate from any caffeine stimulatory effects, right?
01:24:28 Speaker_00
So you can't separate it because it's a complex compound, this Yerba Monte thing. It's got lots of things in it, but also, you know, I've observed you being vocal on social media when people have said, hey, this thing increases GLP-1.
01:24:44 Speaker_00
You quite appropriately, I think, said, wait, you know, Ozempic and drugs like that increase GLP-1 thousandfold.
01:24:55 Speaker_00
When you talk about a food or drink or maybe a supplement increasing GLP-1, it's very unlikely that it increases GLP-1 to that level, meaning,
01:25:04 Speaker_00
unless you're getting into the hundred-fold or thousand-fold increases, probably not right to talk about GLP-1 being the source of any appetite suppressive effect.
01:25:14 Speaker_01
Yeah, that's all correct. So, I mean, I think it's important sometimes to distinguish between pharmacologic and physiologic effects.
01:25:22 Speaker_01
Physiologic is what the hormone naturally does in your body and what can be modulated by natural things like eating a different food. And you might get a twofold change in your GLP-1 by eating a different food, one food versus the other.
01:25:34 Speaker_01
But as we know from those DPP-4 inhibitors, it's not going to really change your appetite because the drugs increase it threefold. These GLP-1 agonists are really a pharmacologic effect, an effect that only happens with drugs.
01:25:45 Speaker_01
a thousand to ten thousand fold higher concentrations of these drugs in your blood than the natural hormone. And so it's just, there's no diet that's ever going to give you that.
01:25:55 Speaker_00
And there's no precedent for it either. So should we be at all concerned about that? I mean, they run clinical trials and address safety, but when you're talking about a thousand fold increase in essentially a peptide hormone, if we were talking about
01:26:10 Speaker_00
different peptide hormone, you know, pick one, you know, oxytocin or estrogen, testosterone, they're not really, you know, broadly speaking, most people would be concerned about thousand fold dosing of something like that.
01:26:24 Speaker_00
And obviously there are clinical indications where that's important. However, my observation of the ever expanding literature on GLP-1 agonists is that there seems to be improvements in like reduction in alcohol consumption.
01:26:43 Speaker_00
And by the way, why would increasing GLP-1 reduce craving for alcohol? It seems like there's an ever-expanding list of things that GLP-1 agonism is good for, but we are talking about, I would say, supra-physiological levels when one takes it.
01:26:59 Speaker_00
And again, I'm not against it nor for it, I'm just paying attention to the literature.
01:27:05 Speaker_01
That's absolutely right. When you're increasing the level of hormone a thousandfold, you need to be careful, see what's happening. But at the end, it's an empirical question. What does it actually do to a person?
01:27:14 Speaker_01
And it can only be answered through experiments. And I think the nice thing about these GLP-1 drugs that a lot of people don't realize is they've been around and approved since 2005, the earliest ones.
01:27:24 Speaker_01
And even something like Ozempic, which maybe only entered the public consciousness in the last year or two, right? It's been around for seven-ish years, I think. And big clinical trials with these drugs.
01:27:35 Speaker_01
And the evidence so far is that they seem to be incredibly safe. And as you said, not just incredibly safe, but they seem to have all these unexpected health benefits that seem to be in some cases even unrelated to weight loss.
01:27:48 Speaker_01
And so, because of the reasons you mentioned, one of the things the FDA requires from these pharmaceutical companies for diabetes drugs is these large cardiac outcome trials.
01:27:58 Speaker_01
So basically where you measure stroke and where you measure heart attacks and death from any cardiac cause. Big trials, like 20,000 people, four years, cost like a billion dollars to run.
01:28:09 Speaker_01
And the data from the semaglutide, the Ozempic trial, came out last year and, as expected, reduced the rate of heart attacks, strokes, all-cause mortality, according to cardiac, for cardiac reasons.
01:28:23 Speaker_01
But what's really surprising was a lot of that seemed to happen before the people even lost weight.
01:28:27 Speaker_01
So there was already a difference between the placebo group and the semaglutide group before the people on the drug had lost a significant amount of weight.
01:28:34 Speaker_01
And there was no correlation between the amount of weight they lost and how well they were protected from heart disease.
01:28:40 Speaker_01
And that's led many people to think that some of these effects actually could be due to other things that GLP-1s are doing that we didn't expect. And so one thing is there's an idea emerging that they are anti-inflammatory.
01:28:51 Speaker_01
So these brain regions, the areoposteum and the NTS,
01:28:55 Speaker_01
are also really important for this reflex, known as the inflammatory reflex, that basically starts with the vagus nerve, goes to these brain regions in the brainstem, and then goes back down to the body to basically suppress, to prevent out-of-control inflammation.
01:29:10 Speaker_01
And so it's thought that these drugs perhaps have an anti-inflammatory effect that explains some of that.
01:29:16 Speaker_00
Sounds like the patent on these drugs just got extended by another hundred years. That's a that's a that's a bio pharma joke.
01:29:24 Speaker_00
I mean, just to put context on it, drugs can be patented and sold as a commercial version and not as generic versions until the patent runs out unless companies are able to find another.
01:29:35 Speaker_00
um approved clinical use in which case it can be re-marketed only as a brand name not generic version so a lot of companies once they do the safety testing and uh and all given everything they put into the r&d into the research and development there's a very big incentive to
01:29:53 Speaker_00
not necessarily finding new drugs, but finding new uses for the same drugs and not allowing generic versions into the picture.
01:30:01 Speaker_00
And that's why it's likely to be based on these, what sounds like additional uses of ozempic related compounds a long time before there's generic ozempic available.
01:30:14 Speaker_01
I think it will be a while, I don't know the exact status of the patents, but I'm guessing it's gonna be a while before there are generic versions, but there's a lot of competition coming.
01:30:22 Speaker_01
So every major pharmaceutical company, or almost every major pharmaceutical company now has a GLP-1 program. And some of them are really exciting, actually.
01:30:32 Speaker_01
So, I mean, the general trend in this area is what people call GLP-1 Plus, which means you take the GLP-1 agonist, which is already giving you 15% weight loss or so, And then you add additional things to that to give it additional properties.
01:30:46 Speaker_01
So one compound is from Eli Lilly, which makes this other, so there's this other drug on the market that we haven't talked about, but Terzepatide, which is known as Moonjaro for diabetes and Zepabound for obesity, which is even better, really in almost every respect, a better drug than Ozempic.
01:31:00 Speaker_01
So people lose more weight, so it's about 21% weight loss at a year. Fewer side effects, at least at comparable doses. That seems to be because this other drug, terzapotide, it has two targets, not one.
01:31:15 Speaker_01
So whereas ozempic is just GLP-1 receptor agonist, terzapotide is a dual agonist of GLP-1 and this other incretin that we talked about, JIP, G-I-P.
01:31:27 Speaker_01
And it seems like having that JIP agonism actually acts as an anti-nausea effect, that sort of counteracts some of the nausea caused by the GLP-1 in the area post-treatment.
01:31:36 Speaker_01
There are JIP receptor neurons in the area post-treatment, this nausea center. It allows you to crank up the dose of the GLP-1 agonism even further while you're suppressing the nausea and just get even more weight loss.
01:31:47 Speaker_01
So now, talking about the future, things that aren't available yet but will be in the next couple of years.
01:31:52 Speaker_01
So Eli Lilly, the company that makes this drug, Tears Appetite slash Mujaro, they have a triple agonist that's in phase three clinical trials now. So this is now three hormones in one.
01:32:03 Speaker_01
It's the GLP-1, which all these drugs have, the GYP, which is the anti-nausea component, and then glucagon itself. And so there's these three hormones all combined in one pill. What the glucagon does is it increases energy expenditure.
01:32:18 Speaker_01
This is a well-known effect of glucagon. And so you're basically eating less, your nausea isn't as bad, and now you're just burning more calories at baseline. And the results from this drug are incredible.
01:32:30 Speaker_01
There's been one phase two trial published and people lost 25% of their body weight at the end of the, I think it was 48 week period. And they were still losing weight. So we don't know where the end point, we don't know what the maximum is.
01:32:42 Speaker_01
So there are bigger, longer trials going on now to figure that out. But at that point when you get beyond 25% body weight, You're talking about basically bariatric surgery, right? Which is currently the best thing we have.
01:32:52 Speaker_01
You know, like these surgeries people do to- Stomach staples.
01:32:55 Speaker_00
Yeah.
01:32:55 Speaker_01
Removing a portion of the stomach. Removing a portion of the gut. So really, it's a pharmacologic version of bariatric surgery.
01:33:02 Speaker_01
The other one that I think is really exciting, there's this compound from Amgen, it's called, it's just right now, it's just a code. It's like AMG-133. But it's like terzapatide in the sense that it targets both GLP-1 and GYP. So it's a dual targeted.
01:33:18 Speaker_01
But unlike trizepatide, which activates the GYP receptor, this amgen compound inhibits it. And for reasons that people don't understand, either activating or inhibiting this receptor causes you to lose weight.
01:33:30 Speaker_01
So still a mystery, but a lot of debate about what's going on there. But the way this amgen compound activates the GYP receptor, or inhibits the GYP receptor rather, is that it's an antibody.
01:33:43 Speaker_01
So all these other things were peptides, but this is a much bigger sexual protein. This is an antibody.
01:33:47 Speaker_01
And because it's an antibody, it has a much longer lifetime, even than something like semaglutide, which is seven days, so it lasts like a month in the blood or something.
01:33:55 Speaker_01
And so you can give people monthly injections of this, and they lose dramatic amounts of weight. And then at least in this initial trial, at the end of this, they stopped and people maintained the weight loss for six months. That's impressive.
01:34:07 Speaker_01
Potentially because of the long lasting effects of this antibody or potentially because of other things that we don't understand. So, and those are just two, there's all sorts of other crazy things happening.
01:34:15 Speaker_01
So really, I think it's just created this explosion of interest in pharma. Basically it's one of these things, once you see that something can be done, all of a sudden that changes everyone's perspective. And so now,
01:34:27 Speaker_01
Obesity drug discovery has gone from something that 10 years ago Everyone wanted to stay away from because there were so many nightmare stories about drugs that turn out to be not safe Till now everybody's sort of all in on this.
01:34:37 Speaker_00
Yeah, I remember in college the fen-fen debacle where a diet drug was released and people had cardiac issues start dying So it was pulled from market and then it was essentially a quiet field for a long time In part to bring us back into the brain and in part because it's directly relevant to what we've been discussing
01:34:56 Speaker_00
about Ozempic and GLP-1, there are other neurons in the brain that regulate feeding. And there are other peptides involved in appetite control for which I would say niche communities have started to indulge in.
01:35:12 Speaker_00
And by the way, people were taking GLP-1 analogs long before they were FDA approved in kind of niche communities.
01:35:19 Speaker_00
These aren't communities I'm a part of, but every once in a while I'll stick an ear into one of these communities and hear what people are taking. And a big thing right now
01:35:27 Speaker_00
in these communities is the use of other peptides that are in the melanocyte-simulating hormone pathway. And you mentioned melanocorticone. receptor containing neurons.
01:35:42 Speaker_00
Could you tell us a little bit about what these neurons do in the absence of any pharmacologic stimulation? And then why it would be that people would perhaps stimulate these pathways with these drugs?
01:35:56 Speaker_00
Not that we're recommending that, but I do think that given that some of these neurons are also involved in sexual behavior and FDA approved for the treatment of hyposexual function in women, things like that.
01:36:08 Speaker_00
There is FDA approval for some of these compounds that they're interesting hypothalamic neurons that are starting to gain more attention and that I predict based on their potential involvement in feeding appetite and weight control are likely to enter the picture with more prominence in the not too distant future.
01:36:26 Speaker_01
So alpha MSH, as scientists call it, the hormone you were just referring to, is a product of the POMC gene. So in the same way that we just talked about, glucagon can be processed into different things.
01:36:38 Speaker_01
In some cells it's made into the glucagon hormone, in other cells it's made into GLP-1. POMC, that gene can be processed to produce different hormones. And one is alpha MSH, which is very important for feeding control.
01:36:49 Speaker_01
And so these POMC neurons, they're in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus, the same region where these AGRP neurons I talked about earlier are located. And there's sort of these,
01:36:59 Speaker_01
these two sets of neurons have opposing effects on body weight regulation. And so alpha MSH inhibits food intake and AGRP neurons promote food intake.
01:37:10 Speaker_01
And where they converge is at this receptor, the melanocortin-4 receptor, which is important for body weight regulation. And so alpha MSH is an agonist, it turns on that receptor, and the AGRP peptide is an antagonist, it turns it off.
01:37:26 Speaker_01
And so, you know, there's a lot of human genetics, as I mentioned earlier, implicating this pathway in body weight regulation. There've been a lot of efforts over many years to turn alpha MSH into a drug. And it's been very difficult.
01:37:42 Speaker_01
There is one drug that's now approved. It's called, I think, I'm gonna get the name wrong. It's like set melanotide or something like this. It's an MC4 receptor agonist.
01:37:52 Speaker_01
It's mainly used in relatively small populations of people that, for example, have mutations in this pathway. It's not used as a widespread, as a drug. And the challenge has been really side effects.
01:38:04 Speaker_01
So there's an increase in blood pressure that happens sometimes with these medicines, partly because this pathway controls not only appetite, but also autonomic tone, sympathetic nervous system activation.
01:38:18 Speaker_01
So it's just taking a step back from everything we've talked about today.
01:38:24 Speaker_01
I talked about this short-term system and the long-term system that controls energy balance and body weight, the long short-term system in the brainstem, the long-term system in the hypothalamus, the long-term system being leptin and alpha MSH and AGRP.
01:38:36 Speaker_01
When I was coming up, learning about this stuff 15 years ago or 20 years ago, the dogma was you could only affect body weight through the long-term system, by manipulating the long-term system.
01:38:49 Speaker_01
Because any manipulation you did of the short-term system in the brainstem, the animal would just compensate. And there were these famous experiments where they would take CCK, which is a hormone just like GLP-1,
01:38:59 Speaker_01
injected into rats, injected several times a day, and CCK is known to decrease the size of meals, and it would decrease the size of meals, but the rats would never lose any weight because they would just eat more meals to compensate, and they would just perfectly compensate by eating more meals.
01:39:10 Speaker_01
And so the lore was, well, it's just impossible. The animal will always compensate unless you hit this body weight set point regulating area, which is the hypothalamus, the long-term system.
01:39:20 Speaker_01
But then what the pharmaceutical industry discovered, which I guess maybe shouldn't be so surprising, but I guess it was to some people, is that if you just hit that receptor, that short-term system, 24 hours a day, seven days a week, and never let it stop, then you will lose weight, right?
01:39:32 Speaker_01
And so the short-term system alone is enough to cause body weight regulation. On the other hand, The long-term system with alpha MSH and AGRP neurons and POMC and all this stuff has been a challenge to pharmaceutically target.
01:39:44 Speaker_01
As you know, leptin we discussed didn't really work. And so I think there's gonna be, as you mentioned, a re-emergence of interest in considering this other pathway now that we've seen the success of the GLP-1s.
01:39:58 Speaker_01
And I think one area where it may emerge is in considering their combination, perhaps at different stages of weight loss.
01:40:05 Speaker_01
So perhaps, you know, what would make a lot of sense scientifically, I don't know if it'll work in practice, is that you would take a GLP-1 drug to lose the weight, and then at some point you might stop that drug.
01:40:15 Speaker_01
and switch to a more hypothalamus-centered leptin-based drug to keep the weight off.
01:40:20 Speaker_01
So basically use the GLP-1 drug to force yourself to lose the weight and then use the leptin hypothalamus-based drug to sort of say, okay, this is our new body weight set point. Let's not resist this weight loss that's happened.
01:40:32 Speaker_01
Whether that will actually make sense practically is hard to say because, you know, the GLP-1 drugs have just a lot of benefits even beyond weight loss. People might not want to stop taking them, but that's one idea. Very interesting.
01:40:43 Speaker_00
I'd love to talk about dopamine. We hear so much about dopamine being involved in pleasure.
01:40:50 Speaker_00
I like to think I've had at least a small level of impact in convincing people that it's also involved in perhaps mostly involved in things like motivation, different forms of learning, and lots of other things too, folks.
01:41:03 Speaker_00
Dopamine does lots of things. It's even expressed in the eye and controls adaptation to light. So it does lots of things, but it certainly, is believed that dopamine is involved in our either craving for food or pleasure from food.
01:41:17 Speaker_00
What's the real story on dopamine as it relates to food and eating behavior? You had a beautiful paper published in Nature entitled, and we'll put a link to this in the show note captions, dopamine subsystems that track internal states.
01:41:31 Speaker_00
And I love this paper for a variety of reasons, but if you could give us the high points of your discoveries on dopamine as it relates to feeding, I think, I know, in fact, that people would find it very illuminating.
01:41:43 Speaker_01
Sure, fantastic. So yeah, the question of what dopamine does with respect to feeding is a great question and a difficult question, I think, to answer. There's a lot of misconceptions.
01:41:54 Speaker_01
I think the evidence is dopamine probably isn't so much involved in the pleasure of food, that taste, the hedonic experience. One reason we think this is because you can make mice, Richard Palmutter did this decades ago, that don't have any dopamine.
01:42:09 Speaker_01
And they still show the same sort of effective responses to food. So you put something sweet in their mouth, they like it, right? What dopamine seems to be important for with respect to food is two things.
01:42:21 Speaker_01
One is the motivation to engage in work to get food, particularly when it's high levels of effort. So if you ask a mouse to press a lever to get a pellet of food, if it doesn't have any dopamine, it won't do it.
01:42:35 Speaker_01
And if it has low levels of dopamine, it'll just work a little bit. So dopamine is important for sort of energizing action and motivating you to engage in hard tasks. The other thing that dopamine is really important for is learning.
01:42:46 Speaker_01
It's important for learning about which cues predict something useful for the body, and feeding is a central example of that.
01:42:54 Speaker_01
And what that paper of ours is about is the idea that this learning actually happens on two different time scales for two different kinds of cues.
01:43:03 Speaker_01
So what we almost always talk about with dopamine and learning, which is important, is learning about how external cues in the environment predict something like food availability.
01:43:12 Speaker_01
So you see a McDonald's sign, and you know that that means there's some tasty food in there. And so dopamine is involved in that process of learning what that external cue means. And that's a very fast timescale process.
01:43:28 Speaker_01
In the laboratory, for example, we will play a tone and then give an animal a sip of a solution that has calories in it, for example, and it can learn the association between that tone and that food is going to be available if they're separated by a few seconds, but that's all.
01:43:44 Speaker_01
And that's a dopamine-dependent process. But there's a second sort of much slower timescale learning about food, which isn't about where I go to get a hamburger.
01:43:53 Speaker_01
but rather about what the experience of eating the food, the oral sensory experience, its taste, its flavor, its texture, how that relates to the post-ingestive effects.
01:44:01 Speaker_00
And I should say that this seems extremely relevant to the McDonald's example, because in your experimental situation, the tone is analogous to the golden arches of the McDonald's sign. Exactly.
01:44:12 Speaker_00
But in my experience, and forgive me, but most of the food that I've consumed from McDonald's does not taste good. relative to other like really delicious hamburgers or French fries or something like that.
01:44:25 Speaker_00
I mean, it's, so you're saying dopamine is required to link the signal, the golden arches or the tone to the presence of food at a particular location.
01:44:36 Speaker_00
But not to the experience of pleasure from that food, which squares very well with my experience of McDonald's. And I probably haven't had a bite of McDonald's in 20 plus years, I would have to be pretty hungry.
01:44:49 Speaker_01
I haven't either. And I, it's funny, the golden arches thing is just something that people in neuroscience talks about dopamine use.
01:44:55 Speaker_01
And so now I've started subconsciously just talking about golden arches, even though I also haven't eaten at McDonald's in decades.
01:44:59 Speaker_00
In and out burgers, better tasting from what I understand, probably better sourcing. We're not going to get into all of this in detail, but everyone has their preferences.
01:45:07 Speaker_00
But, but I do think it's interesting because what we're talking about here is related, I think, to
01:45:13 Speaker_00
this notion of highly processed food packaging, the commoditization of food, just the idea that we are drawn to food for things other than the taste that we expect for it. There's all this context.
01:45:25 Speaker_01
So I think an important distinction that people make is the distinction between wanting and liking. I don't know if you've talked about this previously on the podcast.
01:45:32 Speaker_00
Ana Lemke, my colleague at Stanford came on the podcast, talked about dopamine is about wanting as opposed to enjoying.
01:45:39 Speaker_01
Exactly.
01:45:39 Speaker_00
In most cases. Yeah.
01:45:40 Speaker_01
So liking is the subjective hedonic pleasure in the moment of eating it, but wanting is just what's what you want. And these can be uncoupled all the time. You can want things that at the end of the day, you don't actually enjoy it when you get it.
01:45:51 Speaker_01
And I feel like a lot of life is like that. Indeed. And so dopamine is very powerful at making you want something, but not necessarily like it.
01:46:02 Speaker_01
So that's one element, but then there's this other element that is important, but very much less studied, but I find much more interesting, which is how you connect the sensory cues associated with food, its taste, its flavor, its smell, with the consequences for the body.
01:46:18 Speaker_01
And this is so important because so much of whether we like or dislike a particular food or drink is related to its post-ingestive effects. You come to like things, for example, that have calories.
01:46:29 Speaker_01
So this is one of the reasons that adults will eat vegetables and other savory foods that children find disgusting. Even though they're a little bit bitter, you learn through experience, this makes me feel good to eat this.
01:46:37 Speaker_01
And even maybe at a completely subconscious level, there's also a level of learning that occurs. And this, of course, happens with other things like coffee and beer and other things like that.
01:46:47 Speaker_01
And so there's been an idea that this other much slower learning occurs.
01:46:51 Speaker_01
And the reason I say it's slower is because the time between when you taste the food and when it actually gets into your intestine and releases the hormones that might drive this is quite slow, separated by tens of minutes.
01:47:01 Speaker_01
But how that works hasn't been clear. There's been an idea that dopamine might be involved, but it hadn't really received a lot of attention.
01:47:08 Speaker_01
And so we set out to investigate what is the role of dopamine in these post-ingestive responses, and sort of map out for the dopamine system, how does the dopamine system respond, not when you see the golden arches, which is usually the kinds of experiments that have been performed, but rather when you deliver nutrients directly to your stomach, or when you deliver water directly to your stomach if you're thirsty, and so on.
01:47:28 Speaker_01
And what we saw was that there are these different populations of dopamine neurons that are tuned to respond to signals from inside the body. And so there are some that respond when nutrients are in the stomach and intestine.
01:47:40 Speaker_01
There are others that respond in a thirsty mouse when the blood is rehydrated, when you basically satiate your thirst. And we showed that the purpose, or at least a purpose of that activation, is to cause you to learn about
01:47:56 Speaker_01
the effects of what you just ate, basically to create this connection between the flavor of something and its post-ingestive effects.
01:48:02 Speaker_01
So that delayed dopamine signal after ingested food and fluids is sort of reinforcing this connection between the flavor of what I just ate and that it was something good for me.
01:48:11 Speaker_01
One of the sort of interesting things about that paper that was not the direction we initially expected to go in is that for food, I think it's kind of intuitive. There are lots of flavors to food. You have to learn, you know,
01:48:22 Speaker_01
what all these different flavors mean. For thirst, people find it a little less obvious because thirst is just water. Aren't you just born knowing what water is? How do you have to learn anything to do with drinking a glass of water?
01:48:34 Speaker_01
But it actually is a learning question in part because for many animals, probably most animals, thirst is something that's associated with eating, not drinking. There's this study I love of rabbits in New Zealand.
01:48:48 Speaker_01
So there's not a lot of people studying what animals, how they get their fluids in the wild, because who cares, but it's kind of interesting.
01:48:54 Speaker_01
And so in New Zealand, there's this huge rabbit problem, because they're an invasive pest species that was introduced in the 1800s, and they're just eating all the land. And so there's lots of money to study rabbits, to understand their ecology.
01:49:06 Speaker_01
And so a group of researchers did this experiment where they made this big pen outside where they put a bunch of rabbits in this. The rabbits couldn't escape, but they had all their natural food. It was like an outdoor area.
01:49:15 Speaker_01
And they also put a trough of water, so the rats always had access to water, which is a clean water, and they could measure how much water the rabbits drank. And what they basically found is that nine months out of a year, rabbits drink zero water.
01:49:26 Speaker_01
They drink absolutely zero, because they get all of their water from food. The only time they drink is during the winter, when all of the greenery has sort of become shriveled, and then they can't get water from that anymore.
01:49:38 Speaker_01
And so it's just kind of interesting aspect of how many animals are very different from the way we think about ingestive behavior.
01:49:43 Speaker_01
But that fact that animals have to get water from food raises this question, how do they know which foods are rehydrating?
01:49:50 Speaker_01
Presumably they have to learn that because you can't just look at a food and say, if you've never had any experience, oh yeah, this is something that's very water rich and this will rehydrate me when I'm thirsty and this one is not.
01:50:00 Speaker_01
And so James, the graduate student who led this project, basically investigated this by giving mice different fluids and then measuring the dopamine response.
01:50:11 Speaker_01
And he showed there was this delayed dopamine response after the mice had drank the fluids that correlated with rehydration of the blood. So a whole bunch of dopamine neurons get strongly activated when the blood is rehydrated.
01:50:23 Speaker_01
And he hypothesized this might be a signal, this delayed activation of dopamine neurons that allows animals in the wild to learn that food I just ate is rehydrating.
01:50:34 Speaker_01
And so he did an experiment where he basically gave them two different flavors, mimicking sort of the flavors of two different foods, one of which was hydrating and one of which was not.
01:50:43 Speaker_01
And the animals couldn't tell because he infused the water directly into their stomach. And he showed that basically these dopamine neurons are critical for them learning that association.
01:50:52 Speaker_00
So that's the story of that. I love it. And I'll tell you why.
01:50:56 Speaker_00
When I was in college, for reasons that I don't recall, I decided to run an experiment on myself where I would eat one meal that was fairly low water content, like a piece of meat or something with some cheese, you know, what some people call a keto meal, but I wasn't ketogenic.
01:51:14 Speaker_00
I don't even think I knew what a ketogenic diet was at that point. And then the next meal I would have like, a salad and some fruits. And then I would switch back and forth. And I generally would only eat two or three times a day.
01:51:26 Speaker_00
You know, anyway, there's only so many hours in the day. And I found it to be incredibly satiating. And I found that I felt great. And I can imagine any number of different reasons for that.
01:51:39 Speaker_00
And there are these theories that you probably recall that the diet that was being promoted in the nineties where people would either eat carbohydrates or protein separately. Like there was some wackiness out there.
01:51:50 Speaker_00
And as I say that I'm sure I'll get assaulted in the comments. It's probably not wacky. I'm sure there's some, enzymatic basis for why that would be useful. If you enjoy it, go for it.
01:51:59 Speaker_00
I don't have a feeling about it one way or the other, but one thing I noticed was that low water content containing meals, either by virtue of the foods that they include or by virtue of the fact that they're not diluted, so to speak,
01:52:18 Speaker_00
it's a different taste experience to eat those foods than it is to eat like a big salad or something of that sort. In any event, I don't do that any longer. I just sort of stopped, but it was a fun experiment.
01:52:31 Speaker_00
And I think it was efficient because at the time I had very low money as a student. So, you know, generally fruits and vegetables were less costly than meats and things of that sort. But,
01:52:39 Speaker_00
in all seriousness, to what extent do you think humans overeat or undereat depending on the water content of the food?
01:52:48 Speaker_01
It's an interesting question. So, you know, there is this advice that you should, if you're hungry, first drink something, drink some water and see if you're still hungry. And the idea is that
01:52:59 Speaker_01
Perhaps humans can't always, I mean, our interoceptive sense, our ability to sense what our body needs is not perfect. And sometimes we could be confused and we could really be thirsty when we're hungry and hungry when we're thirsty.
01:53:12 Speaker_01
And there's some evidence that that could help. I would say it's probably not a huge effect in most of modern day life, but it's an interesting idea.
01:53:21 Speaker_00
Yeah. This brings us to the topic of thirst, something that your laboratory has worked on extensively and the topic of osmolarity of salt consumption and things of that sort. In broad terms, how do these things link up?
01:53:38 Speaker_00
Meaning, are there instances in which what we really need is salt and we end up eating a bunch of Parmesan cheese?
01:53:46 Speaker_00
I got teased yesterday by my team because occasionally when I'm on the road, I don't like most of the foods available in most airports and stuff. So I'll bring a chunk of really nice Parmesan cheese. I'll just break off a piece and eat it.
01:53:55 Speaker_00
I'll have half a cucumber and I'll have a can of, not a can of tuna, but there are these wonderful jarred filet of tunas that are available that are in olive oil. They taste really good. This is not canned tuna, it's really good.
01:54:08 Speaker_00
And I'd rather eat that in most cases until I can get to a decent meal than like what's put in front of me on an airplane, most of the time.
01:54:18 Speaker_00
So I get teased about this, but I noticed that for instance, sometimes I'll eat the cheese and I think, oh, actually what I really just want is the salt. I really want the salt. I've been drinking a lot of coffee today.
01:54:28 Speaker_00
I've had a couple extra glasses of water. Maybe I'm just craving salt and I'm confused and I'm over consuming this cheese. When in fact, what I'm going for is the salt. As you point out, our understanding of exactly what we need is fairly crude.
01:54:44 Speaker_00
And oftentimes we overshoot the margin, especially when foods are in combination. So salt, water, and let's just say calories. How do we accurately or inaccurately pursue those at the level of biology.
01:55:02 Speaker_01
I'm throwing tough questions at you, but you're- It feels like my qualifying example. Well, there are separate systems, there's thought to be separate systems that control salt appetite, thirst for water, and hunger for calories.
01:55:17 Speaker_01
And so they involve different brain regions for the most part, different neurons, different signals from the body. In general, hunger and thirst are pretty separable.
01:55:28 Speaker_01
I would say the instance where they interact is in phenomena such as dehydration anorexia.
01:55:34 Speaker_01
This is the idea that if I give you some dry food but I don't give you any water, you're gonna eat less food because basically you're gonna get dehydrated and you're gonna decide I need to preserve my fluid balance even if I eat less calories.
01:55:49 Speaker_01
So we prioritize hydration. Yes, you will at some point. The level of survival. At some point you will prioritize hydration. That's related also to the concept of prandial drinking.
01:55:58 Speaker_01
So many animals, including humans, drink most of their water during meals because you basically want to counteract the osmolites that are in your food.
01:56:06 Speaker_01
Salt balance, though, and thirst, so the thirst for water and the desire for salt are much more tightly linked because the purpose of both systems is to maintain the composition of the blood at its right concentration.
01:56:19 Speaker_01
So you want to have the right osmolality of the blood, which you can just think of in simple terms as sort of the total concentration of all the salts. It's a little more complicated than that, but it doesn't really matter.
01:56:31 Speaker_01
And you also specifically need to maintain the sodium concentration at the right level. And so there are really powerful, innate mechanisms that drive both. I think thirst is very intuitive to people.
01:56:45 Speaker_01
You get dehydrated, you lose water, you become thirsty. And we know now that there are a very small set of neurons in a few brain regions that control that. And the way they're thought to work is they contain osmosensors, so they contain basically
01:57:05 Speaker_01
These neurons are sensors for the osmolality of the blood and they're activated when the blood osmolality gets too high. And it's incredibly sensitive system. So you can perceive an increase in your blood osmolality of 1% as the sensation of thirst.
01:57:17 Speaker_01
So remarkable. Yeah. That's how critical it is to maintain salt balance. Exactly, exactly. And so you get to 10% increase in blood osmolality and you're in extreme discomfort and 20% you're like in the hospital.
01:57:29 Speaker_00
So if I took, let's just say a, a half an ounce sip of seawater, inadvertently. It's extremely aversive. It's like, like you just, you want to drink some non-salty water, some nice clean water.
01:57:48 Speaker_01
Yes, exactly. Immediately. Yeah. So I should emphasize that there's two components to the fluid homeostasis system, to the water homeostasis system. One is this desire to drink. But the other is, of course, the kidney.
01:58:01 Speaker_01
And so the reason that drinking the salt water won't put you in a really bad situation is your kidney would then filter out a lot of that salt and cause you just to pee it out, and then you'd be fine. And so those two work in balance.
01:58:11 Speaker_01
The kidney's controlling how much of the salt gets reabsorbed into the blood, and then this desire for thirst, this desire to drink, allowing you to replenish the blood with water at various intervals.
01:58:24 Speaker_01
Yeah, I mean, the experiments led to the discovery of this third circuitry are amazing. It was this guy, Bengt Andersen, working in the 1950s. And he just had this hypothesis that there was an osmosis sensor in the brain, right?
01:58:33 Speaker_01
Which is very, I think, you know, there was some evidence to suggest it, but it was not really, really strongly supported at the time by the data.
01:58:40 Speaker_01
And so he took these goats and he just started infusing small amounts of salt into various places in their brain, reasoning that if there was an osmosis sensor- Sorry to chuckle, I was wild.
01:58:50 Speaker_00
I mean, I wasn't chuckling in jest, like I feel for the goats, I feel for everyone involved in that experiment, but what a wild experiment, just to put salt directly into the brain?
01:58:59 Speaker_01
Yeah, concentrated saline solution, yeah. And he found this tiny region in and around the hypothalamus that if you infuse salt in this region, the goats will drink like eight liters of water in five minutes, just crazy, right?
01:59:13 Speaker_01
And so he reads, okay, this must be the osmosensor. And then he went back and stimulated those neurons, it's just the same thing, the goat just drinks like crazy.
01:59:21 Speaker_01
And so now we know there's this couple small regions around the hypothalamus, one's called the subfornicle organ, another one's called, well, it doesn't really matter, but basically, that have these osmosensors.
01:59:32 Speaker_01
One of the interesting things about the regulation of fluid balance is you face some of the same challenges we just talked about with the regulation of food consumption, which is that you have this behavior, this ingestive behavior that leads to replenishment of the body, but there are these delays.
01:59:46 Speaker_01
So if you're thirsty and you drink a glass of water, it can take on the order of 20 to 30 minutes for the water to be absorbed into your blood.
01:59:53 Speaker_01
for the blood to be rehydrated, and then for these osmosensors that Bank to Anderson discovered in your brain to be, sort of sense that and return to normal activity.
02:00:03 Speaker_01
But of course, if you had the experience of drinking a glass of water, you know that you can quench your thirst within minutes, right? So how does that work within seconds even?
02:00:11 Speaker_01
So one of the other sort of experiments we did early in my lab was to ask that question by basically recording for the first time the activity of these neurons that Bengt had discovered by putting the salt at the goats.
02:00:21 Speaker_01
We went back into them now in mice. Mice have the same neurons. You have the same neurons. And recording their activity when a thirsty mice drinks, it asks what happens. And what we saw was that the neurons don't wait until the blood is rehydrated.
02:00:34 Speaker_01
They also don't do what the AGRP neurons do, meaning they don't look at the water and predict how much water they're going to drink.
02:00:40 Speaker_01
But instead, they get a signal from the mouth, which every time the mouse takes a lick of water, their activity goes down a little bit. And basically, they track in that way the volume of water that's passed through the mouth.
02:00:51 Speaker_01
They also get the signal from the blood, really relaying the osmolarity of the blood, and they compare these two.
02:00:55 Speaker_01
And basically, when the mouse has drank enough in order for the animal to predict that the blood osmolality is going to return to normal, then the animal stops drinking.
02:01:04 Speaker_00
Beautiful. It's just beautiful. It's incredible.
02:01:08 Speaker_00
essentially predicting with, it sounds like a high degree of accuracy, how much water one needs to drink, linking it to the pleasure and of ingesting good clean water under conditions where we're thirsty in anticipation of adjusting blood osmolarity in 20 minutes.
02:01:25 Speaker_00
I mean, it's, yeah, I mean, this is the kind of thing that just, it delights me because it just means that the brain as a predictive organ is just, is so,
02:01:37 Speaker_01
It also explains some sort of funny aspects of thirst that you may have noticed from everyday experience. So one idea is that just cooling your mouth can sort of quench your thirst, right?
02:01:48 Speaker_01
So if you're in the hospital and you're not allowed to drink any fluids, they'll give you ice chips to suck on to sort of quench your thirst. So why is that?
02:01:54 Speaker_01
And so one idea is that perhaps because water is usually cooler than your body, that sensation of water passing, it always cools your mouth. And so you learn, or maybe it's innate,
02:02:03 Speaker_01
that just cooling of my mouth means that basically I'm going to be rehydrated. So Chris, this was an experiment done by a graduate student named Chris Zimmerman.
02:02:10 Speaker_01
Chris did the same thing where he was recording these thirst neurons, but he put a cold piece of metal on the mouse's tongue.
02:02:15 Speaker_01
And you can see when you do that, these thirst neurons go down in activity and then you remove the cold piece of metal and they go back up. Amazing.
02:02:20 Speaker_01
So a lot of these sort of oddities of everyday experience have to do with how the system has evolved to make the prediction about what's going to happen to the body.
02:02:29 Speaker_00
I mean, few things are as rewarding as the sensation of drinking really nice, clean, cold water when one is very thirsty. When my lab was in San Diego, I used to take my dog hiking in Palomar Mountain, and one day, you know, I really screwed up.
02:02:44 Speaker_00
He was a bulldog mastiff. They overheat easily, and it was a lot warmer than we thought. We ran out of water. It was actually a dangerous situation for him.
02:02:53 Speaker_00
We got down to the bottom of the hill, thankfully, with him still alive, and there's this pump that pumps what is, I was told was spring water and it came out, you know, really cold and you could just see him fill back up with life.
02:03:07 Speaker_00
I filled back up with life knowing he was filling back up with life. And it was, it was unlike the kind of reward that one experiences with food when you're hungry. It's like that basic critical need for water.
02:03:20 Speaker_00
under conditions where you're clearly dehydrated is like nothing else. It's delicious in a way that no food is delicious.
02:03:27 Speaker_01
I would like to actually say something about that. So that distinction you made is really interesting between hunger and thirst. So when you stimulate these neurons that make an animal thirsty, the mice hate it.
02:03:36 Speaker_01
They will do anything to avoid something that artificially makes them thirsty. So we can artificially stimulate these thirst neurons, create a state of virtual thirst. They'll lever press hundreds of times to make it stop.
02:03:46 Speaker_01
The same neurons, the neurons I talked about that control hunger, the AGRP neurons, they actually don't care so much. They won't really do much of anything to shut them off.
02:03:55 Speaker_01
That raises the question, why do the animals eat then when you stimulate the hunger neurons? And we think the primary thing that the hunger neuron stimulation does is it makes food itself more attractive.
02:04:04 Speaker_01
It makes the food more delicious, more of an attractive motivational magnet. It makes the experience of eating more pleasurable. but it is not itself the most unpleasant state. At least the mice aren't willing to do that much.
02:04:14 Speaker_01
Whereas for thirst, I think, you know, dehydration and thirst is really just unpleasant and animals just want to avoid that. And so I think the distinction is very real. I think there are two different motivational mechanisms for hunger and thirst.
02:04:25 Speaker_01
Hunger is mostly about the reward of food. Thirst is mostly about, this is just really unpleasant.
02:04:31 Speaker_00
And removing that unpleasant. Exactly. And you had a paper, which I was going to ask you about, so I will, entitled the forebrain thirst circuit drives drinking through negative reinforcement. And I'm guessing that.
02:04:43 Speaker_00
illustrates exactly the point you just made. So it's a forebrain circuit.
02:04:47 Speaker_00
So does that mean that there's some elements of learning and cognition around this, or are we broadly speaking about the forebrain, for instance, the hypothalamus being in the forebrain?
02:04:55 Speaker_01
So, yeah, it's interesting. So the third circuit, for whatever reason, is mostly in the forebrain. So the neurons that, so we talked about the NTS and the area post-strema being important for hunger and signals from the gut.
02:05:08 Speaker_01
The ariposteum is a circumventricular organ, meaning it's outside the blood-brain barrier. There's only a couple of these in the brain. The neurons that control thirst are located in the two circumventricular organs in the forebrain.
02:05:20 Speaker_01
One is called the subfornicle organ, the other one is called the OVLT, but they're just acronyms. So why it evolved to have the thirst neurons more in the forebrain and the neurons that sense nutrients more in the hindbrain is a little bit unclear.
02:05:34 Speaker_01
And so there is definitely an element of learning, but a lot of this is those neurons are also just directly sensing the blood and sensing changes in both the concentration of salt in the blood and then also hormones like angiotensin that drive thirst.
02:05:46 Speaker_00
I was going to ask you this earlier, but it seems appropriate to ask now. A colleague of mine at Stanford in the psychology department, Dr. Ali Crum,
02:05:54 Speaker_00
who studies mindsets, has done some interesting experiments where people are told that a given milkshake is calorically dense. Other people are told that a milkshake is calorically sparse. Both groups independently consume the milkshake.
02:06:11 Speaker_00
And then they measure things like hormone responses in the bloodstream that are associated with satiety. And what she finds is that even hormone responses to the same
02:06:24 Speaker_00
shake, meaning the same amount of calories, fat, sugar, et cetera, can be significantly modulated based on what we're told.
02:06:34 Speaker_00
And it extends into some other, perhaps even more interesting areas in my opinion, whereby if people are told that, let's say a given meal that has a small piece of fish, a serving of vegetables and a carbohydrate is
02:06:48 Speaker_00
Yes, perhaps a little bit calorically sparse compared to what one would normally eat at a given meal, but they're told this is a highly nutritious meal. This is good for you.
02:06:57 Speaker_00
Then just that mere knowledge can drive more satiety, better feelings about the meal. Even I believe I have to double check on this, but as I recall a heightened sense of it tasting really good. So humans are very,
02:07:16 Speaker_00
susceptible to the, in this case, the either inaccurate, in the case of the milkshake experiment, or accurate descriptions of food, meaning they shape our perception of whether or not something is good for us, tastes good or not, and whether or not it leads to more or less satiety.
02:07:35 Speaker_00
And I think this is important given the obesity crisis, to say nothing of these drugs that are coming out, whereby people often associate dieting with deprivation and pain,
02:07:45 Speaker_00
But if they understand that certain foods are nutritious, that can at least partially offset some of the pain of caloric restriction. What are your thoughts on that?
02:07:56 Speaker_01
Yeah. Well, I mean, one thing I've been talking about is how a lot of these circuits are anticipatory. They're making predictions. They're trying to estimate what's happening in the future. And I talked about how these AGRP hunger neurons,
02:08:10 Speaker_01
sort of see the food or get input about the sight, smell of food. And that way predict how many calories the mouse is going to eat. But I mean, this is a mouse, right? This is all based on a mouse.
02:08:19 Speaker_01
And a mouse has, you know, a thousand times fewer neurons than you do as a person, right? So the computational capacity that the human brain has to make these predictions is just vast compared to these mice.
02:08:29 Speaker_01
And these mice are already doing amazing things, right? So when you think about then, what is the human brain able to do in terms of anticipating changes in nutritional state and how,
02:08:37 Speaker_01
information that you're given can change the expected physiologic outcomes. I mean, you're right. I mean, there's just this whole other element that
02:08:47 Speaker_01
It's very hard to study because it's happening in the brains of humans and we can't do these kinds of experiments. But I'm sure that's very important. I mean, so I talked a little bit about these flavor nutrient conditioning experiments.
02:08:58 Speaker_01
These are the experiments where essentially an animal learns to consume a certain flavor because it learns it's going to be associated with nutrients later.
02:09:07 Speaker_01
Sort of the paradigm for how you learn to consume bitter vegetables because they're good for you and you get nutrients.
02:09:13 Speaker_01
So people have also done those experiments in humans and that does work, but what they've discovered is it's very sensitive to what you tell the humans about the thing that they're going to consume.
02:09:23 Speaker_01
So if you put nutritional labels where you show the different numbers of calories, then basically they sort of adjust their expectations and nothing happens.
02:09:29 Speaker_01
So it really has to be that sort of, it's very sensitive to what information you give them before the experiment happens. So I think that's an example of that kind of thing.
02:09:39 Speaker_00
without any pressure for it to be prescriptive, how do you approach eating given the knowledge that you have about food?
02:09:49 Speaker_00
I like to assume that you can sit down to a meal and not think about your AGRP neurons too much or any of that, but given that you have deep knowledge in this, has it shaped kind of how you think about food cravings your own, you don't have to reveal what those are even if they exist, how you observe the eating behavior of others,
02:10:10 Speaker_00
And yeah, how has knowledge shaped your feeding behavior?
02:10:16 Speaker_01
Well, I try not to think too much about my AGRP neurons when I'm eating. I would hope. I would hope. I think the circuitry is so complex, and we're just beginning to see what's happening.
02:10:29 Speaker_01
use that kind of information at this stage where we're just beginning to prescriptively.
02:10:33 Speaker_01
But I think there is a set of basic recommendations from physiology and neuroscience, very simple things that you've probably talked about with people on your podcast before, for sort of shaping your diet to be healthier, to limit food intake.
02:10:48 Speaker_01
So one we've already talked about is limiting consumption of ultra-processed food, eating more whole foods for lots of different reasons, because they're more satiating, because they're
02:10:58 Speaker_01
don't have this sort of engineered palatability that causes you to overeat.
02:11:02 Speaker_01
Another big one, which I'm sure you've talked about with some of your guests is protein consumption, making sure you get adequate protein consumption, both because there's this concept of protein leveraging.
02:11:11 Speaker_01
So if you don't eat a minimum amount of protein, that's going to cause you to eat more calories just to try to achieve that minimum amount of protein.
02:11:17 Speaker_01
Also just because protein is more satiating and also because there's this idea of thermic effect of food. And so you basically burn more calories metabolizing protein than sugar or fat.
02:11:26 Speaker_00
How about consumption of fluids during meals? I've heard it said before that we're not supposed to consume too many fluids because it's going to dilute the enzymes that allow us to digest our food. I've heard other people say that's a complete myth.
02:11:40 Speaker_01
That's a myth, I think. Humans don't have a perfect capacity to determine whether they're hungry or thirsty. And so drinking water will ensure you're not eating because you're thirsty. And so there's no idea of diluting it.
02:11:58 Speaker_01
I don't think that meant anything. Distention itself, even though water provides a very limited distention signal, the expansion of your stomach and intestines is one important way that you terminate feeding.
02:12:09 Speaker_01
And so there is some component of that where you can get distention just from drinking water.
02:12:14 Speaker_00
I say it's- Sorry, I blurted out. Interesting, because I didn't realize that fluid consumption only provides a limited signal for distention. Well, it's not fluids, it's water.
02:12:23 Speaker_01
And so the idea is that that you can fill your stomach up with fluids, but the rate at which fluids empty out of your stomach depends on their calorie content.
02:12:32 Speaker_01
So basically, if you drink water, it empties very rapidly into your intestine and then goes through your intestine and is gradually absorbed. If you drink something like a glass of orange juice, it will empty much more slowly.
02:12:42 Speaker_01
And if you drink something that's really high in fat, really high in calories, it will empty extremely slowly over hours. And that's because there's a negative feedback loop from the intestine that controls gastric emptying.
02:12:51 Speaker_01
So as those first nutrients leave your stomach and enter your intestine, that produces hormones that go back and then slow down the rate of gastric emptying. And the purpose for this is that you don't want nutrients entering the intestine too fast.
02:13:05 Speaker_01
That's really unsafe. It feels very unpleasant. And it's just, your intestine can only metabolize nutrients so fast. And so if there's calories, then it slows down gastric emptying a lot, but water just kind of goes through.
02:13:18 Speaker_00
What a beautiful system. There's regulation at every point. Hypothalamus, brainstem, gut, the rate of emptying based on the difference between water and orange juice. It's just awesome.
02:13:34 Speaker_01
And that's part of the reason I think it's so hard to outsmart the system, right?
02:13:37 Speaker_01
Because, you know, these neurons are making predictions based on the sight and smell of food, but then the gut is doing its own thing, it's calculating it separately and relaying that information.
02:13:46 Speaker_01
So at every step there are these checks, basically they're just confirming that what you thought happened the first time actually was really going on.
02:13:53 Speaker_01
And it's, which makes sense because it's so important for survival, these homeostatic systems are the product of, you know, so much natural selection.
02:13:59 Speaker_00
Which I think at least partially explains why thousand fold increases in peptide hormones like GLP-1 are required to see significant long lasting changes in weight. Exactly. Because the system is so strongly regulated. Exactly, exactly.
02:14:15 Speaker_00
It's hard to beat homeostasis. And hard to beat it safely, but it sounds like You're more or less optimistic about where that whole field of, let's call it anti-obesity drugs, is headed.
02:14:29 Speaker_01
I'm very optimistic. I mean, I think that you couldn't have asked for more so far at this stage with these GLP-1 drugs. Incredible weight loss, unexpected health benefits. really safe as far as we can tell.
02:14:44 Speaker_01
I mean, it's always possible that some new side effect will emerge, but these drugs are in millions of people, and they've been in a lot of people for a long time now, and nothing seems to have shown up. So I'm very optimistic.
02:14:57 Speaker_01
And I think even beyond that, just now that the pharmaceutical industry has reinvigorated to investigate this question, there's so many different, people are gonna, in five years, people have so many different options.
02:15:05 Speaker_01
It won't just be Ozempic or Rojaro. There'll be five different, 10 different drugs that they can choose from.
02:15:11 Speaker_01
that have slightly different side effect profiles, slightly different efficacy, perhaps used for people with slightly different metabolic conditions.
02:15:17 Speaker_01
And so it'll really be a whole palette of medicines you can take that will adjust your physiology and hunger.
02:15:23 Speaker_00
And it's amazing how well it squares with the understanding of the basic biology. And that's a perfect opportunity for me to really just say what is
02:15:38 Speaker_00
in my mind and clearly in the minds of everyone listening and watching, which is thank you so much for this absolutely encyclopedic and exceptionally clear explanation of feeding and thirst and salt regulation and these new drugs that are, you know, in everyone's minds and everyone's hearing about.
02:15:58 Speaker_00
I've learned so much today, I know everyone else has. You run an incredible laboratory, I've tracked your career for a very long time.
02:16:06 Speaker_00
Every paper is spectacular and you're in a very competitive field and you've contributed in enormous ways to our understanding of these important processes. And I don't just say that as a formality, I know that to be true given that we
02:16:19 Speaker_00
you know, are from the same field and have known each other for a long time. And I'm familiar with your work at a deep level. Today has just been an absolute privilege and a gift to learn from you. And I know everyone feels the same way.
02:16:35 Speaker_00
So thank you for taking time out of your busy research schedule and the other important areas of your life to come here and educate us all. I learned so much basic and practical knowledge and I know everyone else did as well.
02:16:47 Speaker_01
Thank you so much. Thank you, this has been really fun. I'm really glad we had a chance to do this. We talked about some of my favorite topics, so it's always a pleasure and to talk with another neuroscientist about these things is fantastic.
02:16:57 Speaker_01
Well, please come back again.
02:17:00 Speaker_00
Meanwhile, thanks for everything you do. All right, thanks. Thank you for joining me for today's discussion with Dr. Zachary Knight. To learn more about his research or to support his laboratory's work, please see the links in the show note captions.
02:17:12 Speaker_00
If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. Please also subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify and Apple by clicking the follow button. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
02:17:23 Speaker_00
Also on both Spotify and Apple, you can leave us up to a five-star review. Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast.
02:17:33 Speaker_00
If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast or topics or guests you'd like me to consider for the Huberman Lab podcast, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments.
02:17:43 Speaker_00
If you're not already following me on social media, I am Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. So that's Instagram, X, formerly known as Twitter, Threads, Facebook, and LinkedIn.
02:17:53 Speaker_00
And on all those channels, I cover science and science-related tools, some of which overlaps with the contents of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the contents on the Huberman Lab podcast.
02:18:03 Speaker_00
So again, that's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms.
02:18:07 Speaker_00
And if you haven't already subscribed to our neural network newsletter, our neural network newsletter is a zero cost monthly newsletter that includes podcast summaries, as well as brief one to three page PDFs of what we call protocols.
02:18:20 Speaker_00
Those protocols cover everything from learning and neuroplasticity to dopamine regulation, to improving your sleep. We also have a foundational fitness protocol
02:18:28 Speaker_00
the details, cardiovascular training and resistance training sessions, right down to the details. Again, all of that is available, completely zero cost.
02:18:35 Speaker_00
You simply go to hubermanlab.com, go to the menu tab, scroll down to newsletter and enter your email. And I should point out that we do not share your email with anybody.
02:18:44 Speaker_00
Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion with Dr. Zachary Knight. And last, but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.